Several previous studies reported that human postural sway can be reduced by the application of a particular intensity of nGVS21–33 – an effect that was concordantly attributed to the exhibition of SR within vestibular signal transfer. However, none of these studies so far provided systematic evidence for the latter assumption. In this study, we therefore systematically examined whether SR-like changes in body sway across nGVS amplitudes can be actually observed in young healthy individuals. The exhibition of SR in a particular system is typically indicated by a noise-induced modulation of the system's output that follows a pseudo-bell-shaped performance curve with increasing noise intensity, which peaks at a particular optimal noise amplitude where the system's performance becomes optimally enhanced. In accordance with previous reports, we found reduced body sway (for all three tested balance conditions and AP as well as ML directions) at particular nGVS intensities in almost all participants. However, across the entire range of applied nGVS intensities, individual and grouped body sway modulations exhibited more or less random fluctuations and, in most cases, did not exhibit consistent performance curves. This observation suggests that, at least in young healthy individuals, postural improvements at particular nGVS amplitudes are likely to result from inherent variations in the performance metric (i.e., static body sway) rather than being caused by vestibular SR.
Vestibular feedback cues play a minor role during quiet stance on a fixed surface, where postural adjustments predominantly rely on somatosensory and - to a lesser extent - visual cues.35,41 We therefore focused on stance conditions that pronounce the role of vestibular feedback by (a) withdrawal of visual cues (eyes closure) and (b) by manipulating the proprioceptive reference to the Earth vertical through sinusoidal surface tilts or sway referencing. Even under these conditions we did not observe convincing evidence that nGVS induces SR-like reductions in body sway. Our observations are certainly limited by the focus on a young and healthy cohort, in which peripheral vestibular processing presumably operates at a near-to-optimal level and might thus leave little to no potential for externally induced improvements. It is thus conceivable, that SR-like improvements in vestibular balance control might only be observable in the elderly or in patients with vestibular hypofunction, where age-related or pathological vestibular hair cell degeneration has been associated to a decline in peripheral vestibular signal processing.42,43
However, SR-like effects using nGVS have been reported also for young healthy individuals. Galvan-Garca and colleagues observed in about 75% of young participants, that nGVS-induced modulations of the vestibular threshold for passive motion perception were compatible with SR and followed a bell-shaped performance curve with optimal improvement at a particular intermediate nGVS intensity.17 Analogously, it was demonstrated that nGVS effectively lowers the threshold to induce vestibulospinal reflex responses in about 90% of young participants.44 Thus, the question arises why SR-like behavior can be found in perception and simple reflexes, but not in standing balance.
One reason could be differences between the processing of vestibular cues in ego motion, sensorimotor reflexes, and balance regulation. For instance, vestibulo-ocular reflex thresholds apparently differ from thresholds for vestibular motion perception, in particular in the low frequency range,45,46 and exhibit different response dynamics to vestibular stimulation or visual-vestibular conflict.47,48 Differences become even more apparent when comparing processing of vestibular cues at the perceptual and reflex level to that during balance regulation. The reasons for this lie both in the intrinsic multisensory nature and the complex feedback control mechanisms, which constitute postural control. Accordingly, vestibular input in postural control becomes considerably filtered, distorted, or even masked by the interaction with other sensory cues (proprioceptive, visual), the feedback dynamics of the postural control system, the body’s low-pass characteristics, and intrinsic noise in neural processing and muscular activations.49 In line with this, current models of postural control, assume that vestibular cues only become involved in balance regulation after multisensory fusion (in particular with proprioceptive cues) at a late processing stage that is close to the behavioral output.50,51 Thus, comparisons between nGVS effects on different vestibular-related functions and output measures need to be considered carefully.
Another reason could be that psycho-physical estimates of vestibular perceptual thresholds, which were previously shown to be responsive to nGVS,17 are designed to be particularly robust against intra-individual performance variations and yield excellent test-to-retest reliability.52,53 In contrast, test-to-retest variations in standing balance are considerable.54,55 Thus, inherent trial-to-trial variations in the examined performance metric rather than nGVS-induced effects might govern the response dynamics of static body sway across the range of applied nGVS intensities. However, earlier studies reported nGVS-induced reductions in body sway measures in the order of 10–40%.23,25 The trial-to-trial variability of sway measures from our current recordings had a standard deviation of 2% for quiet stance, 17% for sway-referencing trials, and 20% for sinusoidal platform tilt trials. Thus, at least the test-to-retest reliability during quiet stance would have been high enough to identify the previously reported 10–40% nGVS-induced improvements.
Finally, we observed that significant and consistent nGVS-induced body sway responses started to occur in ML direction for noise RMS intensities at and above 142 µA (approximately corresponding to nGVS peak-to-peak amplitudes of 500 to 700 µA). In accordance in nonhuman primates, neuronal detection thresholds of primary vestibular afferents for GVS applied on the bilateral mastoid processes – a setup analogous to ours – were estimated to lie between 400 to 600 µA peak-to-peak.56 This suggest that vestibular SR in young healthy individuals with intact peripheral vestibular information processing, should be triggered, if any, by nGVS at amplitudes below these estimated peripheral thresholds. This assumption corresponds to the observation made by Galvan-Garca et al. that optimal nGVS-induced reduction in vestibular perceptual thresholds occurred at nGVS peak-to-peak amplitudes at or below 500 µA.17 In contrast, previous reports on nGVS-induced improvements of postural sway at stimulation amplitudes way above these estimated detection thresholds (e.g. 1000 µA21,26,33) are therefore unlikely to be attributable to vestibular SR. In these instances, reductions of body sway presumably rather result from a postural stiffening/stabilization response in the presence of an external induced vestibular disturbance as opposed to an effective facilitation of vestibular balance regulation.