In this work, we explored associations among cerebral cortical thickness, FC, and cognitive impairment in PD. By combining structural and functional connectivities, our findings suggest that alterations in cortical thickness and resting-state FC may contribute to cognitive decline in PD. In the present study, We used regions presenting cortical atrophy as ROIs for the first time, show that atrophy in cortical thickness may result in dysfunction of resting-state functional connectivity. (Achard, Salvador, Whitcher, Suckling, & Bullmore, 2006; Tononi, Edelman, & Sporns, 1998). In addition, we found increased connectivity between the cortical atrophic brain areas and the cerebellar regions in PD with cognitive impairment, which suggests that the cerebellum in particular may be involved in cognitive dysfunction in PD. Furthermore, we associated the decrease in cortical thickness with disease progression in PD patients.
Our data on cortical thickness in PD-associated cognitive impairment are consistent with previous studies showing widespread temporal, frontal, and occipital lobe cortical thinning(C. H. Chen, Lee, & Lin, 2020; Hwang et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2014; Mak et al., 2015; Trufanov, Odinak, Litvinenko, Rezvantsev, & Voronkov, 2013). We also found widespread cortical thinning, involving the superior temporal, lingual, fusiform, and insula regions, which points to impairment in multiple cognitive domains. The temporal lobe is closely related to memory and also affects executive function and visuospatial function through the frontotemporal and occipito-temporal loops(Gratwicke, Jahanshahi, & Foltynie, 2015). Lingual and fusiform regions are posterior visual cortical components, and their atrophy correlates with visual perceptual dysfunction(Goldman, Williams-Gray, Barker, Duda, & Galvin, 2014). Insular lesions in human patients have been shown to impair performance on tasks requiring cognitive flexibility(Hodgson et al., 2007). Our results found that cortical thinning in the left superior temporal, left fusiform, and right temporal pole areas positively correlated with global cognitive decline. This is consistent with the regions known to be involved in higher-order cognitive functions. Previous work(Zarei et al., 2013) also found a positive correlation between MMSE scores and cortical thickness in the anterior temporal, dorsolateral prefrontal, posterior cingulate, fusiform, and occipitotemporal cortex. In addition, longitudinal studies(Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al., 2012; Williams-Gray et al., 2009) have linked gradual thinning in the temporal-occipital cortex to deterioration that can eventually lead to dementia. These considerations suggest that the decrease in cortical thickness may be a promising marker for predicting cognitive decline in PD(Zarei et al., 2013).
The underlying mechanisms behind the cortical thinning that we and others have observed remains unclear, and several hypotheses have been formulated to explain it. First, progression of PD is associated with progressive cortico-striatal circuitry dysfunction, which can be tracked by monitoring glucose metabolism in cortical regions using positron emission tomography(Braak et al., 2003). Thus, dysfunction of projections between cortico-striatal regions may lead to chronic “disuse” of the cortex, leading in turn to cortical thinning. Another hypothesis is that abnormal phosphorylation of α-synuclein and Tau occurs at cortical synapses in PD patients with cognitive dysfunction(González-Redondo et al., 2014), which contributes to cortical mitochondrial dysfunction, leading to neuronal death and, potentially, cortical atrophy(Ferrer, 2009). A third hypothesis is that cortical thinning arises not from neuronal death but from cellular shrinkage and reduction in dendritic arborization(Morrison & Hof, 1997). Indeed, in PD-MCI, the topographic distribution of cortical thinning is consistent with the regions where hypometabolism and hypoperfusion occur(Abe et al., 2003). This leads to reduced size of neuronal cell bodies, reduced dendritic arborization and/or loss of presynaptic terminals, which can result in cortical atrophy.(Braak, Rüb, Jansen Steur, Del Tredici, & de Vos, 2005; Pellicano et al., 2012)
Although the atrophy changes in our patients were relatively limited, they affected cortical regions in areas important for information integration(Achard et al., 2006; Tononi et al., 1998). Analysis of normal cerebral cortical thickness revealed the existence of a group of posteromedial cortical areas that form dense interconnected brain networks(Hagmann et al., 2008). Executive control, default-mode, and salient networks are thought to be mainly related to cognitive function, and these networks depend on, and regulate each other, to ensure normal cognitive function(Pievani, de Haan, Wu, Seeley, & Frisoni, 2011). Cortical atrophy of brain regions at important nodes leads to disruption of brain network connections and dysfunction, which gives rise to cognitive impairment in PD(Tononi et al., 1998). This observation was confirmed in our resting-state FC analysis using atrophic brain regions as ROIs. Our results showed that PD patients with cognitive impairment showed weak FC in insula with executive control and default-mode networks. The PDD group showed weak FC in superior temporal with executive control and default-mode networks; weak FC in insula with the salient network; and weak FC in fusiform with default-mode and executive control networks. The insula is an important node of the salient and ventral attention networks. Moreover, dysfunction in the loops of midbrain ventral tegmental area and the insula contributes to executive dysfunction in PD(Oades & Halliday, 1987), and specific disruption of projections to the insular cortex contribute to worsening executive impairment and PDD. This may result because insula cannot effectively recruit other cognitive networks, such as the fronto-parietal network(Gratwicke et al., 2015). In this way, our study and literature establish strong correlations between atrophy of the insula cortex and progression to PDD. Similarly, our work supports the idea that atrophy of the temporal lobe, lingual, and fusiform regions disrupts the default-mode, executive control, and salient networks, thereby contributing to cognitive impairment in PD.
In our study, PD patients with cognitive dysfunction presented stronger FC between the cortical atrophic brain areas and the cerebellum than healthy controls or PD-NC patients. This implicates the cerebellum may be particularly involved in adaptive changes that occur in PD with cognitive dysfunction in response to local cortical atrophic change. The cerebellum is known to influence motor activity and cognition through the cerebello-thalamo-cortical circuit(Middleton & Strick, 2001). Our patients showed alterations in FC involving voxels related to executive control and default-mode networks, and these alterations likely affect executive ability, visuospatial function, memory, and attention(Li, Su, Li, Jin, & Chen, 2018). To overcome these defects in executive ability and visuospatial function, PD patients may compensate by strengthening their abilities in certain functions (e.g. volition, planning, and purposive action) and in self-referential processing (e.g. recollection of past experiences). This may explain why our PD patients with cognitive impairment showed increased cerebellar FC with executive control and default-mode networks. Similarly, we previously found that PD patients with visual disorders showed stronger FC involving the cerebellar vermis X, visual networks and default-mode networks than patients without visuospatial disorders(Yin et al., 2021). Patients with visual disorders also showed increased FC within the intracerebellar network between the vermis X and left cerebellum 8, and left cerebellum 9 (Yin et al., 2021). The connectivity in another work showed stronger connectivity between the cerebellum and frontoparietal networks in PDD(Zhan et al., 2018). These observations strongly implicate the cerebellum in cognitive dysfunction in PD. Further work should examine whether PD patients strengthen FC between the cerebellum and other networks as a compensatory mechanism.
Our data indicate that the decrease in mean cortical thickness in superior temporal, precuneus, insula, and fusiform regions may be useful for assessing PD severity, confirming previous observations(Burton, McKeith, Burn, Williams, & O'Brien, 2004; Mak et al., 2015; Zarei et al., 2013). PD symptoms and disease severity are typically assessed using the UPDRS score and H-Y staging(S. Chen et al., 2016). UPDRS-III scores in PD patients negatively correlate with the thickness of the left and right fusiform gyrus and left temporal pole(Zarei et al., 2013), while H-Y stage negatively correlates with the thickness of the posterior cingulate cortex and the temporal cortex(Gao et al., 2018). The anatomical distribution of these cortical thinning is consistent with changes in cortical glucose metabolism and perfusion decreases observed in the early stage of the disease, which gradually extend to multiple cortical regions as the disease progresses(Borghammer et al., 2010; González-Redondo et al., 2014). These results further illustrate the feasibility of using MRI biomarkers to assess the pathophysiology of PD motor dysfunction.
Our study presents some limitations. First, despite the rigorous exclusion of MRI data affected by head motion and other preprocessing steps to reduce motion artifacts, such artifacts may still have influenced the results. Second, due to motor dysfunction and poor cooperation of PD patients, comprehensive cognitive assessment was not performed, so our study could not provide more detailed understanding of the characteristics and progression of cognitive dysfunction. Third, the sample was relatively small, and the cross-sectional design prevented us from determining causal relationships. Our findings should be verified and extended in large, longitudinal investigations.