This study showed that having a diverse physical activity pattern in early childhood is associated with higher levels of objectively measured PA seven years later.
DPA at age six was positively associated with MVPA and FMS at age nine. MVPA and FMS at age nine were also positively related to MVPA at age 13 but to a lesser degree than DPA at age six. Furthermore, the association between DPA at age six and MVPA at age 13 was not significantly mediated by FMS or MVPA at age 9 and there were no longitudinal associations from FMS at age six to MVPA at age nine or at age 13.
Although not a high beta value, considering that the association between DPA at age six and MVPA at age 13 was observed over a period of seven years, the β-value of 0.22 is noteworthy. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have investigated the longitudinal association between DPA and MVPA. As we initially hypothesized, and as theorized in the concept of physical literacy[5], DPA at a young age is important for later levels of MVPA. However, the association between DPA at age six and MVPA at age 13 was not significantly mediated by FMS. This is quite surprising as it has been theoretically argued that DPA contributes to well-developed FMS, which then enable participation in many PAs [5, 6]. The non-significant mediation could be due to that the assessment of FMS was rather narrow (mainly postural and locomotor skills). However, the observed associations between early DPA and later PA could also be an indication that early DPA results in inspiration to choose more widely when deciding how to be physically active later in life, or/and that skills more specific to the physical activities available to and undertaken by children and youth in their everyday life than perhaps general motor skills might be important to overall PA. A diverse pattern of PAs would most likely lead to an embodied memory of a varied and broad number of activities. Future studies, e.g., with an experimental intervention design, should investigate the association between childhood DPA and PA and the underlying mechanisms.
DPA at age six was associated with FMS at age nine and 13. We did not find any longitudinal associations between MVPA (at age six or at age nine) and FMS at an older age. As described in the introduction, there is no clear evidence for MVPA as a predictor of FMS [10]. Results from our study have expanded on this sparse knowledge, showing that DPA at age six is important for FMS at a later age, even when controlling for MVPA. The results indicate that FMS does not develop solely by taking part in high amounts of MVPA, but by engaging in many different types of activities at an early age. It has been suggested that FMS needs to be taught, practiced and reinforced, especially later in childhood[21]. Interventions targeting FMS development in PE curriculum has shown to be effective [46], but the enduring effect on FMS or PA after intervention has not been demonstrated. While acknowledging the impact of teaching FMS through an instructional approach, DPA may be an effective, accessible and sustainable way to develop FMS in early childhood years.
To our knowledge four other studies have investigated the longitudinal associations between FMS and PA [7, 19, 20, 47] in samples similar to our sample (between approximately 4–10 years at baseline). Generally, it is difficult to compare results from studies investigating FMS and PA, as many different measurement batteries are used to assess FMS and self-reported measures of PA are still common. A meta-analysis concluded that correlates of FMS differ according to the operationalization of FMS[10]. Barnett and colleagues [19] showed in a study of 276 school children (mean age of 10 years) that manipulative skills were associated with self-reported PA six years later. Because the KTK battery used in our study does not assess manipulative skills, comparisons should be considered with caution. Two other longitudinal studies of the association between FMS at an early age (4–6 years) and later levels of PA are not consistent. Lopes and colleagues [20] assessed FMS in 285 children (mean age of six years) using the KTK battery, and PA was assessed (self-reported) over four consecutive years. The authors observed that children in the highest FMS tertile at baseline showed the smallest drop in levels of PA up to four years later compared to the middle and low FMS tertiles. This result indicates that FMS at a young age is important for later PA. In a study of 207 children, McKenzie and colleagues [47] assessed FMS at ages four, five and six and self-reported PA at age 12. They observed no association between childhood FMS and PA at age 12 and discussed the limitations of the battery used for assessing FMS, as it consisted of three single tests (lateral jump, catch a ball, and balance on one foot). In our study, we also did not find FMS at age six to be associated with PA at age 13, while however FMS at age nine was.
In a seven year longitudinal study, Lima et al. [7] found reciprocal longitudinal associations between FMS and time spent performing vigorous PA. However, this study also showed a mediation of the association of vigorous PA and FMS by cardiorespiratory fitness. Interestingly, Lima et al. [7] observed a stronger association between vigorous PA and FMS than between MVPA and FMS, highlighting the importance of vigorous PA in the development of FMS during childhood. It is possible that the non-significance of the longitudinal associations of early MVPA with later FMS in our investigation can be partially explained by the importance of more vigorous PA on FMS development[14].
Strengths and limitations
It is important to keep in mind that the results that are presented in this study should be viewed cautiously, as the DPA measure is not previously validated or extensively studied. This is especially the case since some of the results of this study contradict previous studies. However, on the other hand, the results of this study also illustrate the necessity of future studies developing and applying validated measures of DPA and investigates its importance for life long PA participation.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the relations between diversity of childhood PA and longitudinal development of FMS and MVPA. The longitudinal design of seven years with two follow-ups and the objective measurement of PA improve the credibility of the results. Furthermore, the participants were followed during childhood and early adolescence, when foundations of PA behaviors are formed that seem to continue until adulthood [48]. Even though the study has an observational design, the associations observed are highly robust to dropout. Even though the data used are relatively old, the associations have general validity, as it is unlikely that the relationship between DPA in childhood and MVPA in adolescence would have changed since the data was collected in 2008.
Although accelerometers, compared to self-reported measurements, are considered a valid instrument for assessing PA in children and youth, some limitations are associated with this method. First, the accelerometers were not worn during swimming. Most of the intervention schools included swimming in the extra PE lessons; thus, this extra activity, due to the method of measurement, was not recorded. Second, due to the memory capacity of the accelerometers, they only gave a three- to four-day snapshot measure of the children’s daily PA at age six and nine.
Additionally, the accelerometers did not measure participation in cycling, which contributes a substantial amount of PA in this population of Danish children.
Another limitation in this study is that we did not control for the cluster effect. The decision not to adjust for the cluster effect was made in order to use structural equation modeling. Structural equation modelling has been shown to be beneficial in investigating longitudinal associations while avoiding selection bias by using full information maximum likelihood estimation to impute missing values. Thus, the structural equation model analysis should be noted as a strength of the study despite the aforementioned limitation. Although our analysis indicates that the sample with full data is similar to the sample with missing data, an additional limitation was the amount of missing data in the primary indicators and outcomes.
The complexity of the model and number of parameters lead us to the decision of not having any latent variables in the structural equation model[44]. Future research could investigate and develop the items and measurement model behind the DPA measure in more depth.
Furthermore, another limitation of the study is that we did not measure DPA at nine years of age, and perhaps larger β-values could have been observed from DPA at age nine for MVPA and FMS at age 13. It would also be possible to explore is cross-sectional results differs in comparison to longitudinal findings. It may be, that DPA is only possible for individuals when FMS are well developed. Future research should investigate whether FMS and/or MVPA at age six is associated with DPA at an older age. Future research should look into the interrelationship among all the variables from a more explorative perspective. Furthermore, future research could also explore whether the associations are different for boys and girls.
Additionally, we advocate that future research develop and adopt a validated measurement tool for DPA. A suggestion for measuring DPA in children may possibly be via ecological momentary assessment[49, 50] over the course of a week. Although being a subjectively instrument, the ecological momentary assessment holds several advantages. With this instrument it would be possible to explore the variety of activities, durations and settings were the child’s PA is undertaken during a week. To date, an objectively method to measure diversity and context of PA is still to be developed.
Implications for practice
These results propose a new dimension for PA-related health recommendations for children, which currently only highlight the importance of being physically active at a moderate-to-vigorous intensity. While the health benefits of PA are well-documented [51], the long term effect of interventions aiming to increase PA amongst children are at best moderate. Focusing interventions on promoting DPA may have greater potential, as it addresses the underlying domains important for supporting PA levels during the whole life course [5].
Several studies show that the amount of daily MVPA is associated with several health outcomes in young children [2], but the optimal amount of MVPA for children and young people is a debated topic. However, if DPA at age six increases the likelihood of being physically active in adolescence and maybe even later in life, then more focus should be placed on encouraging children to engage in a diverse range of PAs. Viewed through a life course perspective [52], our results indicate that investments in helping children engage in a wide variety of PAs is more important for later PA than solely increasing the time spent in MVPA in childhood.
Interventions targeting DPA in early childhood should be conducted to test its potential for maintenance in PA behavior across childhood and adolescent years. Some countries, Canada in particular, have already incorporated the importance of diversified physical activity as an important aim of their physical education curriculums [53]. Our study supports this focus for physical education in schools and perhaps even for kindergarten activities. In addition, agents involved in leisure-time PAs for children, such as sports clubs and associations, should consider the importance of providing diversified PAs rather than focusing on a single sport. The debate and studies regarding early specialization vs. diversification have mostly centered on whether it is more beneficial for later performance levels to engage in a diverse range of activities (i.e., DPA) instead of focusing on one specific sport during childhood [54]. The results of this study show that diversification also appears to be beneficial for general activity levels later in life. Another structural aspect of the importance of children’s daily PA is the environment in which the child lives and interacts, e.g., the number of play facilities available, which seems to be important for activity levels [55]. It seems likely that the diversity of play facility types available is one aspect that can enable DPA in childhood.