The importance of stress, defined as the mental and physical reaction to personal or environmental stimuli that are potentially threatening 1, and its possible harmful influence on both the mental and physical health of an individual has long been established 2,3. Research has consistently shown that, when endured for a prolonged time, stress negatively impacts both the onset and progression of a variety of illnesses such as coronary heart disease, depression, and anxiety disorder 4,5. Given this repeatedly reported link between stress and disease, throughout the last decades a significant amount of research has been dedicated to better understanding the various pathways by which stress affects the different biological systems of humans, and which environmental factors lead to the presence of stress throughout the life of an individual 6,7. Research has identified psychosocial stress as one of the most important forms of stress throughout an individual’s life given its strong link with the development of psychopathology 8. Psychosocial stress, present in either unpredictable or uncontrollable social situations which are deemed unpleasant or threatening 9, has obtained its prominent position due to the abundance of social interactions throughout daily life 8, 10–12.
The role of the brain in the perception of stimuli as stressful and its reaction to stressors as the controlling agent of the following stress response has been a central focus of psychosocial stress research 13–15. Initially, brain activity related to psychosocial stress has been studied mainly with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and multiple brain regions have been identified that are involved in the psychosocial stress response. Cortical regions commonly found are the anterior insula (often coactive with parts of the inferior frontal gyrus such as the pars triangularis and pars opercularis), the anterior and posterior cingulate gyrus (ACC, PCC), and the orbitofrontal cortex (for various systematic reviews and meta-analyses, see 13,16−20). Subcortical regions such as the (para)hippocampus, thalamus, lentiform nucleus, caudate nucleus, putamen, and amygdala are also consistently reported to be involved 13,16,19. Aside from fMRI, electroencephalography (EEG) has been employed increasingly throughout recent years for the investigation of psychosocial stress-related brain activity, with the most commonly researched characteristics of the EEG signal being band power (i.e., the amount of activity within a specified frequency range). The most reported results from these studies are the reduction of power in the alpha frequency range (8–13 Hz), and (although not significant in our recent meta-analysis) an increase in power in the beta frequency range (13–30 Hz).
A variety of paradigms have been developed for the investigation of psychosocial stress that can be used within the imposed limitations of the employed neuroimaging method. Although all paradigms employ a psychosocial stressor (e.g., negative feedback and peer comparison in the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (MIST 21), social exclusion in the Cyberball paradigm 22 or social-evaluative threat in the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST 23)), these psychosocial stressors are often accompanied by other stressors such as cognitive stressors (e.g., imposed time limits or task demands). This co-occurrence of stressor types makes it difficult to directly link the measured neural activity to the unique social aspect of the employed paradigm. In a recent article, Ehrhardt and colleagues (2021) have explicitly investigated the contribution of various individual stressor types (cognitive effort, time pressure, and social-evaluative threat) to changes in alpha and beta band power of frontal electrodes (F7, F3, Fz, Fpz, F4 and F8). The sobering results from their analysis have shown that the employed psychosocial stressor (social-evaluative threat, the fear of being judged negatively) does not significantly alter power in either the alpha or beta band, indicating that results attributed to the social component of a stress paradigm instead seem to reflect changes in cognitive processing 24. Although this implication is highly significant for the research field, psychosocial stressors may alter neural activity in ways not detected by frontal alpha or beta power. Sensor level-derived EEG measures are known to be affected by volume conduction, understood as the spreading of electrical signals from a single brain source throughout the head 25,26. Psychosocial stressor-induced changes in specific brain regions might therefore not be sufficiently registered by sensor level-derived EEG measures or can be overpowered by other spontaneous brain activity. The usage of EEG source imaging, which projects the signals measured at the electrodes back to the neural sources within the brain 27, and the corresponding source space is therefore of special interest. Aside from source level power measures, functional connectivity (FC, the study of temporal dependence between spatially distinct neural events 28) measures might also capture changes induced by psychosocial stressors and thus give more insight into the neuronal psychosocial stress response.
To investigate whether purely psychosocial stressors affect source level-derived EEG indices, we developed a paradigm where participants were exposed to a psychosocial stressor while keeping co-occurring stressors such as time pressure or task demands constant between both conditions. Participants were instructed to solve Raven’s matrices of different levels of difficulty 29. After each matrix, participants received (comparative) feedback which was manipulated to induce psychosocial stress. In the control condition, participants received neutral feedback (i.e., the participant performs on par with other individuals), and in another condition, the negative condition, negative feedback (i.e., the participant performs (increasingly) worse than other individuals). Time limits were kept equal between both conditions and to further eliminate possible interferences of the task itself, only data collected during the feedback exposure were analyzed. To evaluate whether the applied stressor was successful in eliciting a stress response, electrocardiography (ECG) data and state questionnaires, self-assessment manikins (SAM), were also collected throughout the study. The SAM contains two scales: arousal (degree of activation due to the stimuli, from low to high) and valence (experienced emotional reaction to the stimuli, from negative to positive).
The research questions of the current study are threefold. Firstly, we investigated whether the psychosocial stressor elicits a physiological and mental response from the participants. We hypothesized that in the ECG signal, similarly to other psychosocial stressors, we would find an increase in sympathetic reactivity, identified by an increased heart rate acceleration, during the negative-, compared to the control condition 30–32. We further hypothesized that in the SAM, in line with prior research, an increase in the arousal scale and a decrease in the valence scale would be found. Secondly, we tried to reproduce the results found by Ehrhardt and colleagues (2021) and therefore computed frontal theta, alpha, and beta power at the sensor level, and compared the negative to the control condition. We hypothesized that similar to those results, no changes due to a psychosocial stressor alone would be found. Finally, we investigated whether the purely psychosocial aspect of a stressor would be effective enough to affect source level-derived EEG measures. Therefore, we investigated the cortical regions commonly found in fMRI research (i.e., the anterior insula, ACC, PCC, precuneus, and orbitofrontal cortex; see above) and computed both their band power (theta, alpha, and beta) and the functional connectivity between them. Functional connectivity was estimated using amplitude envelope correlation (AEC), a robust connectivity measure 33,34. Given previous fMRI research, we hypothesized an increase in beta power in the anterior insula and an increase in alpha power in the precuneus and PCC 16. We had no specific directional hypotheses regarding the functional connectivity estimates.