Our results find significant secular increases in height and brain structure along with improved memory performance with advancing decades of birth. Brain structures also appear to be increasing at a rate slightly greater than height. Further, improved memory performance accompanying advancing decade of birth appears to be mediated (at least partially) by increasing hippocampal volume after adjusting for age, sex, and education. We hypothesize that larger brain volumes indicate improved “brain health”34, 35 and potentially greater “brain reserve”36–41 that could explain the declining incidence of dementia as previously reported in the Framingham Heart Study5 and further supported by improved verbal memory performance associated with greater hippocampal volume found in this study.
Our findings of consistent enlargement of TCV, cortical gray matter and hippocampal volume with advancing years of birth spanned dates from 1902–1985. Confounding by association of age at MRI with decade of birth was addressed through sensitivity analysis. Results from these analyses found that stepwise restriction of the age of participants, first to 45–74, and then to 55–65 years, where age-related differences in brain volumes were minimal, showed consistent increases in brain size measurements with advancing decade of birth, even when the decades of birth were limited to 30 years as occurred with the most restricted analysis (See Supplemental Materials). Analysis of the age-range between 45 and 75 years of age (equal to the middle quartiles of the age distribution or 75% of the cohort) and covering 5 decades of birth, resulted in multiple unique findings (Figs. 4–6). First, TCV volume, like height, increased linearly through the 5 decades for both men and women. Conversely, decades of birth effect were strongest for hippocampal volume for individuals in the 7th decade of life, and somewhat less for those in the 6th decade, at the time of MRI. These birth years from 1930–1950 coincided with times of great stress such as the depression and World War II. Stress is known to affect hippocampal structure and function 42, 43 and it is tempting to postulate these major stressors as potential factors contributing to secular trends in hippocampal volume along with the many other differences lifestyle that occurred over the same time period. Finally, cortical thickness declined with decades of birth as well as advancing age. Declining thickness coincided with increasing TCV as well as cortical gray matter volume and surface area. Although cortical surface area was positively associated with cerebral gray matter, cortical surface area was inversely associated with cortical thickness. Rakic44 describes how the radial unit lineage model could explain evolutionary and developmental aspects of the cerebral cortex that would be consistent with our findings. In this model, expansion of the cerebral cortex occurs through increasing convolutions and expanding surface area while limiting change in cortical thickness. White et al45 extend this concept to “gyrification” of the cerebral cortex, which they note results in a surface area “1700 times larger [in humans] than in shrews, yet the thickness of the cortex is only six times greater”. Like Rakic44, White et al45 emphasize the computational utility of expanding surface area over thickness within the radial unit lineage model that allows for “an optimized compaction of neuronal fibers with an efficient transit time for neuronal signaling”. Consequently, increased cortical gray matter with advancing decades of birth results in increased surface area through increased gyrification. According to the radial unit lineage model, the increased gyrification (and consequent surface area) leads to subtle reductions in cortical thickness as seen in our data. Importantly, both Rakic44 and White et al45 emphasized regional differences in “gyrification” are likely under unique genetic influence46. Such an analysis is beyond the scope of this report but could add further information regarding the biology of cortical gray matter expansion with subtle reduction in cortical thickness as seen with our more global measures.
Memory performance also improved with decade of birth and was significantly associated with all MRI measures except cortical thickness. Improved memory performance was dramatically associated with level of educational achievement, being nearly 1/3 of a standard deviation higher for college educated participants. Memory performance was nearly linearly associated with decade of birth for women but appeared to asymptote somewhat for men (Fig. 6). This too may be associated with achieved level of education for men versus women. Whether this relationship is due to increasing memory performance that leads to increased likelihood of college education or visa a versa is beyond the scope of this report and requires further investigation. Despite these sex differences, improvement in memory performance was partially mediated by increased hippocampal volume that also occurred with advancing decades of birth.
How might these secular effects modify the likelihood of later life dementia? Brain growth begins in utero, increases throughout childhood, and reaches a maximum size in early adulthood, 47–50. TCV is highly associated with brain growth during normal development 40, whereas aging or disease-related brain-volume decrease does not alter TCV. Thus, adult TCV is a stable valid measure for maximal attained brain size, widely used as a proxy for brain reserve10, 51, and is an important predictor of cognition in old age41 including studies that used similar measures of memory performance51. Conversely, cross-sectional, and longitudinal age-related differences in brain volume measures associate with cognitive performance in aging and disease52–54. Hippocampal volume loss, in particular, is considered to be sensitive to early degenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease55, 56. Although absolute volumes do not associate with cognitive ability per se, as illustrated by the fact that women in this study had significantly smaller hippocampal volumes, but significantly better memory performance, loss of brain tissue within an individual is strongly indicative of pathological effects57, which, therefore, may be buffered by larger structural brain development. As noted in our data, while decade of birth had a significant positive effect of cortical gray matter volume, the impact of aging remains essentially unchanged. Consequently, individuals in each decade of birth appear to atrophy at the same rate, but later decades start further away from any ‘threshold’ of atrophy that might manifest as clinical cognitive impairment. Alternatively, and more likely, larger structural brain development may be a surrogate for other environmental processes ongoing during development and early adult life such as increased brain connectivity58. Increased connectivity is consistent with the radial unit lineage hypothesis44 that enables increased neuronal connectivity through cortical expansion and gyrification45. Such increased connectivity could mitigate the impact of age-related diseases on cognitive performance and fits well with the scaffolding hypothesis of cognitive reserve59. Evidence supporting the notion that experience is associated with regional brain expansion can be seen among London taxi-cab drivers who have larger hippocampal volumes compared to same aged individuals60.
While head and brain size are under substantial genetic influence61,46, 62–64, the timeline of effect found with our results indicate that early life environmental influences are also likely substantial contributors, particularly educational achievement6. Life course perspectives emphasize the impact of early life experiences on brain health65, 66 that also translate into larger brain structures67 and reduced risk for later-life dementia through improved reserve68. Similarly, effort to improve cardiovascular health during adulthood69–71 are associated with reduced incidence of cognitive impairment72 and dementia73 indicating that modifying these factors could also serve to improve resistance to late-life dementia74.
In summary, our results indicate that TCV and brain structures have increased over birth years ranging from 1902 to 1985. These differences are coincident and associated with improved memory performance that is partially mediated by larger hippocampal volumes. These findings likely reflect both secular improvements in early life environmental influences through health, social cultural, and educational factors67 as well as secular improvements in modifiable dementia risk factors leading to better “brain health” and reserve32. While these effects are likely to be small at the level of the individual, they are likely to be substantial at the population level adding to growing literature that suggests optimized brain development and ideal health through modification of risk factors could substantially modify the impact of common neurodegenerative diseases such as stroke and Alzheimer’s disease on dementia incidence6, 34, 70. Moreover, taken together with increases in IQ throughout the 20th century75, these secular trends in brain volume may contribute to an overall more cognitively resilient and productive society.
The strengths of our study include the design of the Framingham Heart Study that began in 1948 and has followed a community of individuals with comprehensive health evaluations throughout much of their lifespan and across three generations. The addition of MRI beginning in 1999 enabled quantitative brain assessment across all three generations spanning more than 80 years difference in dates of birth. Obtaining contemporaneous MRI with neuropsychological assessment also enabled brain behavior associations. A high rate of participant participation resulting in a large sample size across decades of birth also enabled reliable sensitivity analyses that might not be accomplished with smaller studies. The duration of observation that includes younger individuals also suggests that this secular trend may be continuing. Finally, the fact that more than 80% of subjects were imaged on just two MRI machines also helped to reduce machine differences that were further reduced using the NeuroCombat statistical harmonization method.
This study, however, is not without limitations. First, and most importantly, the Framingham Heart Study is predominately non-Hispanic White, healthy, and well educated and, therefore, not representative of the more diverse US population. For example, current evidence indicates social-cultural67, 76–78 and health disparities2 which are more common among non-White individuals32 in the US may adversely affect brain health. Second, this is a cross-sectional study that has limited causal inference. Longitudinal analyses showing secular differences in rates of regional brain atrophy would further support evidence of increased brain reserve through resilience to age-related atrophy39. Similarly, we are not aware of another cohort spanning 3 or more decades of birth to validate these findings.
Despite these limitations, we conclude that this study extends current knowledge by showing that secular trends in brain structure are occurring and are associated with improved memory performance. We hypothesize that the larger brain structure reflects improved brain health and is at least one manifestation of improved brain reserve that could buffer the impact of late life diseases on incident dementia. Our results also support the growing literature that emphasizes optimal brain development and ideal brain health as preventive measures to mitigate against rising dementia prevalence among our aging US population.