This systematic review examined the evidence base on effective strategies to address emotional dysregulation and challenging behaviors in ASD. Unlike previous reviews, our focus encompasses both knowledge of evidence-based strategies to address challenging behaviors from the behavioral analytic field and knowledge on evidence-based strategies to support emotion dysregulation from the fields of psychology and psychiatry. We identified 30 evidence-based strategies that appear to have the most empirical support for addressing challenging behavior or/and emotion dysregulation, including 21 NCAEP identified strategies and nine ones that have been commonly used in the field childhood mental health but were unrecognized as part of the 2020 NCAEP report. Two of these previously unrecognized evidence-based strategies – Emotion Regulation Training, training to recognize one’s own bodily, cognitive, and behavioral signs of stress and employ proactive coping or emotion regulation skills to reduce one’s own stress, and Role Play and Practice, actively role playing with others to practice newly learned behavioral skills – were rated as a top 10 evidence-based strategy according to study design. This finding highlights the need to explicitly teach and actively engage children and adolescents with ASD in the recognition of their own emotional states and to help build their “toolbox” of emotion regulation strategies during times of distress, and to role play or practice other replacement behaviors.
Many of the strategies designated as evidence-based in this review targeting challenging behavior or emotion dysregulation do so indirectly through teaching replacement skills and strategies to reduce stress. The focus on skill building rather than punishing challenging behavior represents a departure from previously accepted practices (indeed Punishment was designated as a minimal evidence strategy) and reflects the importance of framing challenging behavior in the context of lagging skills [7,8]. At school, a behavioral episode may indicate that a child needs additional supports for example, if he/she/they demonstrate(s) escape behavior during a math lesson they may need visual supports to explain the math concept rather than a punishment, such as a timeout. By modifying the activity to support the individual the escape behavior will likely decrease. In the home environment, a child that hits their sibling because they want their toy may stop this behavior with parental prompting of requests for the toy from their sibling and positively reinforcing these communication attempts. Another example in the clinic setting, is that a child who self-harms may decrease this behavior through being taught how to recognize the early bodily signs of stress and use calming strategies such as sensory toys to address the child’s sensory needs. The key to addressing the lagging skills in each of these scenarios is to first work out the function that the challenging behavior has for the child.
Indeed, fourth highest on the evidence-based strategy list (Table 2) was FBA, a strategy classified with an evidence Grade A (Fig. 3), which is a widely recognized first step in intervening on challenging behavior in children and adolescents with ASD to determine the behavior’s function. For example, the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA, 1997/2004) requires the use of FBA to develop plans targeting challenging behaviors and improving communication in individuals with disabilities. Although we listed FBA alongside the other strategies, it is best characterized as an assessment procedure for developing a behavior intervention plan, rather than an “intervention” strategy itself. Previous reviews differ in terms of characterizing FBA as a strategy (see different approaches by the NCAEP and the National Standards Project) and previous reviews on challenging behavior in ASD have drawn different conclusions regarding the central importance of FBAs. Some suggest it is not a necessary step as effectiveness does not seem to relate to completion of an FBA [16]. Others, however, suggest better outcomes relate to interventions that include FBA [15,17]. We found that studies with a positive effect on outcomes were more likely to have done an FBA than studies with null/negative effects on outcomes, which is suggestive of the critical importance of determining a behavior’s function before choosing which evidence-based strategy to implement. However important, performing FBAs in the school setting are often complicated by lack of professionals able to assess the child as many schools rely on external behavioral consultants to conduct these assessments. Given the critical importance of determining a behavior’s function to create successful behavioral plans, these results highlight the need for a new implementation approach for conducting FBAs in schools. For example, including professional development and coaching of teachers and other school staff on the data collection and/or analysis of FBAs, which could build on their foundational skills and training in behavioral data collection, could help to avoid the bottle neck that this can cause for developing and actioning behavior plans.
Relatively few studies incorporated emotion dysregulation or emotion regulation outcome measures, and these tended to be more recent studies (since 2015), with two exemptions, [25,26] marking that as a field there is growing emphasis on emotion dysregulation in ASD [7]. No studies incorporated physiological stress outcomes. Given that individuals with ASD often have difficulties expressing their emotions and stress [27–29], such a multimodal assessment approach could help triangulate findings or reveal insights into their behavior and response to intervention [23]. Studies using a transdisciplinary approach to examine both challenging behavior and emotion dysregulation outcomes are still emerging [25,26,30–33]. Future research in the area should aim to incorporate multiple assessment methods and outcome measures.
This review also highlights major gaps in the field in terms of studies covering certain developmental stages, contexts, and groups. Relatively few high-quality studies examined interventions targeting middle or high school students with ASD (though we acknowledge there are ongoing studies targeting this age range [34]; only four strategies were tested in RCTs in high school students, and none were tested in RCTs in middle-school students. There is a milieu of hormonal, social, and emotional changes that take place throughout middle childhood and adolescence, including new challenges and opportunities for experiencing and regulating emotions (or dysregulating) emotions, which may manifest as challenging behavior [35]. Therefore, it is important to consider only the specific evidence-based strategies tested in this age range as evidence-based for this age range, given that most participants included in the review were preschool or elementary school students. These findings highlight the clear need for future research to address this knowledge gap.
Moreover, our review highlighted the lack of female study participants and a lack of interventions delivered via telehealth, though we may expect more of the latter to come given the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. Of note, a significant limitation was the inclusion of predominantly white samples wherever race/ethnicity was indicated in studies. This severely limits the ability to apply the results on what is considered evidence-based on a population-based level, since emotional dysregulation and challenging behavior may have deeper cultural biases and expectations (e.g., what is considered emotion dysregulation or challenging behavior). Future research should aim to include more racial and ethnic diversity in samples to determine if results on evidence-based strategies and considerations around choosing target behaviors generalize across racial/ethnic groups. Overall, these findings suggest the knowledge gained to date about evidence-based strategies to support children and adolescents with ASD in reducing emotion dysregulation and challenging behavior is representative only of a sub-group of younger, less racially/ethnically diverse, mostly male with ASD, but not necessarily applicable to the wider ASD population. Further research is needed to fill each of these crucial gaps.
Interestingly, we found that the proportion of null/negative effects was much lower in single-case design studies compared to group design studies, suggesting there may be publishing bias at play. Authors may choose to publish cases that have a higher rate of success in treatment, so attaining an accurate picture of null/negative effects using single-case design studies may be difficult [36]. This points to the importance of routinely reporting null/negative effects to allow for a more comprehensive and unbiased examination of the literature.
Limitations
This systematic review is not without limitations. First, although the evidence level across all evidence-based strategies within each study was given equal weight in our review, some evidence-based strategies may have been more important than others (the “active ingredients”) in producing positive intervention effects. However, no studies examined discrete evidence-based strategies within intervention packages separately. This, and the use of outcome measures that are not comparable across studies, precludes a fine-grained analysis of specific components within different intervention packages. Examining active ingredients of challenging behavior/emotion dysregulation interventions in children and adolescents with ASD is an important future direction for the field. Additionally, for consistency, we limited coding of the discrete strategies to the articles, or if studies referenced previously peer-reviewed manuscripts in describing intervention components, those manuscripts were also reviewed. However, for intervention packages, this may mean that we missed discrete strategies that are in intervention manuals but not described in the peer-reviewed publications. Finally, our search criteria may have presented biases as grey/unpublished literature and articles not translated to English were excluded. As such, our study is not exempt from publication bias or selection bias as relevant articles (with both positive and null/negative effects) may have been missed.