Chronic pain attributed to osteoarthritis causes significant burden of disease in domestic animal patient populations, with 20% of pet dogs experiencing painful osteoarthritis, affecting on average 11% of their lives (Anderson, Zulch et al. 2020). The assessment of canine osteoarthritis pain is based primarily on the pet owner’s observation of functional changes in the pet’s physical or affective behaviour (Gingerich and Strobel 2003, Brown, Boston et al. 2008, Piel, Kroin et al. 2014). Current approaches to understanding and treating canine osteoarthritis pain focus on the structural characteristics of osteoarthritis, disease modifying treatment approaches and pharmacological analgesia (Bland 2015, Anderson, Zulch et al. 2020). However, the nature of chronic pain is now understood to be more complex than a linear consequence of structural changes and nociceptive input from affected joints.
Pain can be classified based on both the duration (acute and chronic) as well as the physiological mechanism contributing to pain (Abd-Elsayed and Deer 2019). Mechanism-based pain classification includes nociceptive, neuropathic and nociplastic pain. Pain associated with osteoarthritis may be a combination of these pain types. Designing the most effective management plan for osteoarthritis pain requires an understanding which pain mechanisms are involved.
Mechanism-based classification of pain
Nociceptive pain occurs when nociceptors, widely distributed in the joints and surrounding tissues, are triggered by a potentially or actually noxious stimulus (Middleton, Barry et al. 2021). Such stimuli could be related to mechanical pressure or irritation of the articular and periarticular tissue, or a chemical stimulus caused by enzymes associated with joint inflammation. These nociceptive stimuli are then processed by several areas of the brain, including the somatosensory cortex and the limbic system to generate the perception of pain and to either amplify or attenuate pain, and generate an appropriate physiological and behavioural response. In a healthy system, descending nociceptive inhibitory pathways modulate pain through the action of beta-endorphins, enkephalins, and dynorphins on opioid receptors (Kendroud, Fitzgerald et al. 2021), among others. Nociceptive pain tends to respond in a predictable way to analgesics and anti-inflammatory medication.
Neuropathic pain occurs subsequent to lesion or disease of a somatosensory nerve resulting in silent, or mechanically insensitive nociceptor subtypes becoming active in the presence of inflammation. The nerve activation threshold drops, and changes in the dorsal root ganglia cause alterations in the type and number of peripheral receptors ready to sense nociceptive input. If this is not resolved, it may induce an increase in the excitability and receptive fields of central sensory neurons at spinal cord level, in a process likely mediated by the activation of glial cells. This process results in generalised hyperalgesia, which can lead to cortical reorganisation and a reduction in thalamic and prefrontal grey matter in patients with chronic pain, impairing the activity of the descending nociceptive inhibitory pathways to modulate pain (Curatolo, Arendt-Nielsen et al. 2006).
Nociplastic pain is defined as pain that arises from altered nociception despite no clear evidence of actual or potential tissue damage. It occurs where altered functioning of pain-related sensory pathways in the periphery and CNS causes increased sensitivity to somatosensory stimuli, which is experienced by the individual as pain. Nociplastic pain can co-occur with nociceptive and/or neuropathic pain, and is often associated with co-morbidities such as fatigue, interrupted sleep, sensitivity to light and/sound and cognitive problems. Nociplastic pain is the underlying mechanism behind chronic primary pain, where pain is experienced in the absence of any actual or potential tissue damage, and without evidence of somatosensory sensitisation (Abd-Elsayed and Deer 2019). Examples of nociplastic pain syndromes include chronic widespread pain, complex regional pain syndrome, chronic visceral pain syndromes and chronic primary musculoskeletal pain (Fitzcharles, Cohen et al. 2021, Nijs, Lahousse et al. 2021). It is increasingly understood that musculoskeletal pain, such as osteoarthritis pain, can be attributable to multiple, co-occuring pain mechanisms (Kosek, Clauw et al. 2021). The theoretical model of nociplastic pain in animals is recognised in the veterinary literature (Herzberg and Bustamante 2021). Most medications used to treat nociplastic conditions provide only a modest benefit, and adverse effects are more likely to occur in (Fitzcharles, Cohen et al. 2021). Therefore treatment for nociplastic pain syndromes emphasise non-pharmacological interventions.
Time-based classification of pain
Acute pain is pain that occurs immediately after an injury, and is usually nociceptive in nature unless a nerve lesion has occurred, in which case it would be nociceptive and neuropathic. Chronic pain is pain that continues beyond the normal healing process i.e. three months. Despite a general convention to classify pain as chronic when it lasts for three months or longer, pain can be considered chronic as soon as it becomes a maladaptive – it no longer serves a protective purpose and is no longer associated with the actual state of tissue repair [13].
Pain and disability
Chronic pain is associated with, and frequently measured in terms of functional disability in humans (Breivik, Borchgrevink et al. 2008, Dansie and Turk 2013) as well as animals (Gingerich and Strobel 2003, Brown, Boston et al. 2008, Piel, Kroin et al. 2014). However, there is not a consistent correlation between disability and pain (Vina, Ran et al. 2018, Vaughn, Terry et al. 2019, Helminen, Arokoski et al. 2020). In human arthritis patients, including children, it has been demonstrated that functional disability is moderated by physiological, emotional and cognitive factors(Kruger-Jakins, Saw et al. 2016, Louw, Zimney et al. 2016, Coakley, Wihak et al. 2018, Ernstzen, Keet et al. 2022), and caregiver self-efficacy (Woby, Urmston et al. 2007, Ashcraft, Asato et al. 2019, Rondon-Ramos, Martinez-Calderon et al. 2020, Bacardit Pintó, Ickmans et al. 2021). The Fear-Avoidance Model of pain links pain avoidance, cognitive processes and physiological reactivity to pain and disability in human chronic pain patients, including paediatric populations [13]. It describes the contribution of pain-related fear to the development of chronic pain, with growing evidence supporting the role of pain-related fear in pain catastrophising, hypervigilance, increased avoidance behaviours, and intensified pain intensity and functional disability (Leeuw, Goossens et al. 2007).
In animals, the experience of chronic pain also involves negative affective experiences and altered cognitive functioning (Pais-Vieira, Mendes-Pinto et al. 2009, Low 2013, Rochais, Fureix et al. 2016, Cockburn, Smith et al. 2018, Cowen, Phelps et al. 2018, Smith 2019), and similarly affects physiological processes (VMD 1998, Yu, Lundeberg et al. 2003, Kang, Park et al. 2022). Dogs have a cognitive ability structure, or phenotype, that is similar to that found in people, and are able to learn abstract concepts, often through interspecies (human-dog) social learning (Bensky, Gosling et al. 2013, Arden and Adams 2016, Lazarowski, Davila et al. 2021). Behaviour is the primary measure of chronic pain in dogs (Gingerich and Strobel 2003, Brown, Boston et al. 2008, Hielm-Björkman, Rita et al. 2009), but canine behaviour is, in part, a socially learnt phenomenon (Elgier, Jakovcevic et al. 2009, Horowitz 2009), and is mediated by affective states (Mendl, Brooks et al. 2010, Fureix and Meagher 2015, Ahloy-Dallaire, Espinosa et al. 2018). A dog’s cognitive processing and judgement biases of a situation or experience are influenced by the dog’s affective state, which affects behaviour (Starling, Branson et al. 2013, Cimarelli, Schoesswender et al. 2021). Canine affective states are also correlated with physiological changes such as fluctuations in cortisol and oxytocin, as well as dysregulation of heart rate variability, which are all factors associated with poor pain regulation in humans (Evans, Seidman et al. 2013, Vachon-Presseau, Roy et al. 2013, Carlesso, Sturgeon et al. 2016, Telles, Sharma et al. 2016, Tracy, Ioannou et al. 2016, Boll, Ueltzhoeffer et al. 2020, Ferrero, Amri et al. 2021, Paschali, Lazaridou et al. 2021).It is reasonable then to expect that cognitive, social and emotional factors will influence the pain experience of a dog in a way similar to that demonstrated in humans.
The behavioural expressions of the multi-dimensional, subjective pain experience of an individual dog is used as an objective measure of the dog’s pain levels (Mota-Rojas, Marcet-Rius et al. 2021), usually by the pet-parent, often with little awareness by the observer that their own behaviour can have a mediating effect on the behaviour they are observing in their dog. This interplay between the states and traits of the observer (pet parent) and the subject (dog) introduces an intersubjective dimension to chronic pain as being a phenomenon experienced by a dyad.
Many robust studies have been published that explore human-dog interactions from both anthrozoological (the interdisciplinary study of the interaction between humans and other animals), as well as comparative cognition approaches (Pfaller-Sadovsky and Hurtado-Parrado 2021). Studies of animal cognition are methodologically challenging (Miklósi and Abdai 2021, Pérez-Manrique and Gomila 2022), but evidence suggests that the similarities between human and dog social behaviours facilitate interaction and bonding with familiar humans (McGreevy, Starling et al. 2012, Duranton and Gaunet 2018, Grigg, Liu et al. 2022). Some dog-human interactions resemble intra-specific (dog-dog) prosocial behaviours, particularly in terms of affiliative behaviours and the use of body language. However, differences between intra-specific and inter-specific (human-dog) behaviours are observed in other situations, such as behaviour around food, the use of social referencing and rescue behaviour (Nowbahari and Hollis 2010, Quervel-Chaumette 2016, Quervel-Chaumette, Mainix et al. 2016).It has been suggested that novel forms of cognition arise out of human-dog interactions that cannot be explained by the individual species’ ethograms. In other words, behaviours observed during dog-human interactions may differ from behaviours normally observed between members of the same species – dog-dog and human-human (Merritt 2021).
Emotional contagion has been defined as: “The tendency to automatically mimic and synchronize expressions, vocalizations, postures, and movements with those of another person’s and, consequently, to converge emotionally” (Hatfield, Cacioppo et al. 1993). Behavioural synchronisation is an accepted proxy for emotional contagion, as it is an adaptive social phenomenon seen in many animal species (Duranton and Gaunet 2016). Synchronisation increases the chances of survival of each member of a social group by improving predator avoidance, increasing foraging efficiency and promoting social cohesion among individuals. It also helps to maintain pair bonds Suggestions that basic aspects of mimicry, or behavioural synchronisation, are precursors of emotional contagion are supported by neuroimaging studies in humans. (Duranton and Gaunet 2015). Rapid mimicry (RM) and yawn contagion (YC) are two such behavioural synchronisation processes, and have been shown to be socially modulated, occurring more frequently between individuals sharing close relationships.
Yawn contagion is considered to be a primitive form of behavioural synchronisation, although it is not associated with a specific emotional valence. The social modulation of yawn contagion is supported by psychological, ethological and neurological evidence, and demonstrates an association with the level of attachment bond (Palagi, Celeghin et al. 2020). Yawn contagion has been shown to occur in intraspecific relationships in Canid species. Romero et al (2014) found that wolves in their natural habitat yawned more frequently when exposed to conspecific yawning – yawning of other wolves - an observation that was positively linked to the strength of social attachment of the pair (Romero, Ito et al. 2014). There is less agreement that interspecies yawn contagion occurs between dogs and humans, with contradictory results emerging from experimental studies. Yawning is a well-described stress-response in dogs, which poses challenges in an experimental environment where dogs are exposed to novel stimuli, making it difficult to draw conclusions about yawn observations.
Another primitive form of emotional contagion is rapid mimicry. As a foundation for social competence, rapid mimicry facilitates play and reduces escalating aggression by functioning as a communicative signal to indicate shared intent. Mimicry occurs outside of conscious awareness and of voluntary control. Mimicry differs from imitation in that it involves no recognition of a goal associated with the action, and is thought to be generated by the mirror neuron system (Davila Ross, Menzler et al. 2008, Hess and Fischer 2013). The empathic nature of rapid mimicry, particularly during play, is supported by observations of a gradient of rapid mimicry behaviour related to the familiarity of the partner. This suggests that it is possible for two or more individuals who are synchronising their behaviour (i.e. mimicking) to share the same affective state (emotional contagion) (Palagi, Celeghin et al. 2020). The neurological processes underlying mimicry can be explained by the perception-action model (See Fig. 2). This states that the perception of the emotional state of others involuntarily produces a corresponding state in the receiver. This allows for social alignment of non-congruent emotional states, which stimulates reward centres in the brain (Preston and De Waal 2002). Mimicry is the foundation of play-related behavioural synchronisation – through the positive feedback loops generated by play-related behavioural synchronisation, play serves as an unconscious mechanism for reinforcing social affiliation (Preston and De Waal 2002, Palagi and Scopa 2017). Mimicry facilitates a self-sustaining loop that fosters emotional engagement (longer play duration), which in turn increases social closeness and synchronization (familiarity), increasing the probability for mimicry to occur (Palagi and Scopa 2017). Play is defined as all activity which has no immediate benefits, and which involves motor patterns typical of serious contexts (Burghardt 2005). Play may be a tool for reducing stress and improving social cohesion, and as such is often considered an indicator of welfare in dogs (Sommerville, O’Connor et al. 2017, Ahloy-Dallaire, Espinosa et al. 2018). Dogs combine body postures and facial expressions (eye and mouth movements) to convey emotional states (Quaranta, Siniscalchi et al. 2007) as well as motivation to play (Horowitz 2009). The adaptive value of mimicry depends on the ability of an individual dog to discriminate play signals when performed by other dogs.
Emotional contagion appears to be mediated by neurobiological reward mechanisms such as oxytocin release (Zoratto, Sbriccoli et al. 2018, Laviola, Busdraghi et al. 2021). It likely arose as an adaptive consequence of environmental sharing rather than genetic factors, and functions as a flexible social learning strategy (Nakahashi and Ohtsuki 2018). Emotional contagion can result in personal distress: “a self-focused, aversive emotional reaction to the vicarious experiencing of another’s emotion” (Eisenberg and Eggum 2009) or empathy “the naturally occurring subjective experience of similarity between the feelings expressed by self and others without losing sight of whose feelings belong to whom” (Decety and Jackson 2004). Behavioural indicators of empathy are considered to be comfort-offering or helping behaviours in response to another’s distress (Custance and Mayer 2012). While evidence for emotional contagion is widespread in the animal kingdom, it is mostly observed in the context of negative emotional states such as fear or pain. Negative emotional states tend to be easier to detect, and methods for identifying negative emotional states in animals are well-established. Consequently, most reports of emotional contagion are in the aversive domain (Düpjan*, Krause et al. 2020).
The human/dog dyad has been shown to be analogous to parent-child attachments (Prato-Previde, Spiezio et al. 2003, Taggart 2011, Asher, England et al. 2020), therefore it is hypothesised that pet-parent affective/cognitive states and behaviours have similar impacts on physical disability in dogs with painful osteoarthritis as have been observed in parent-child dyads (Mumme, Fernald et al. 1996, De Rosnay, Cooper et al. 2006, Robins, Perron et al. 2016, Bacardit Pintó, Ickmans et al. 2021). To support this hypothesis, evidence is needed to demonstrate that negative emotional states associated with amplified pain experiences can be transmitted from the pet-parent to their dog (emotional contagion). A preliminary search of MEDLINE and the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews was conducted and no current or underway systematic reviews or scoping reviews on the topic of the mediating effect of human caregiver emotional states on dogs’ emotional and cognitive pain processing were identified. Several studies, including one scoping review, demonstrate the beneficial effect of the human-dog relationship on human chronic pain (Horowitz 2008, Markovich 2011, Bradley and Bennett 2015, 2019, Carr, Norris et al. 2020, Nitkin and Buchanan 2020), but no studies evaluate the potential positive effect of this relationship on canine chronic pain.
The aim of this scoping review, therefore, was to evaluate the literature on emotional contagion in the human-animal dyad, with specific focus on the mediating role of human emotion on the affective and behavioural states of dogs that are typically associated with canine pain. These domains include the following: cognitive/emotional processing, behaviour and physiological parameters (Fig. 1). The rationale for selecting these criteria are the interconnected nature of these domains as they relate to pain. We were interested to explore the potential impact of modifying the beliefs, attitudes and behaviour of pet owners on pain-related functional disability in dogs with chronic pain. We conclude by discussing the potential implications of these dyadic phenomena for the management of chronic pain in dogs.
Review question
Aim:
In this review, we will explore the potential implications of human-dog dyadic interactions on chronic pain in the dog, by better understanding inter-species emotional contagion, specifically the human-dog dyad, and its physiological and cognitive consequences.
Our objectives are to determine the following:
-
Affect and effect: How do human affective states mediate canine affective states and cognition as measured by behaviour?
-
Affect and physiology: the measurable effects of negatively valenced inter-specific interactions on canine physiological variables
-
Affect and environmental adaptation: how does the valence of human-dog interactions influence a dog's ability to deal with novel (and potentially frightening) situations?
-
What are the potential implications of these dyadic interactions on chronic pain in the dog.