Demographics
A total of 139 participants completed the survey (78 women, 52 men, mean age = 48.9 ± 15.5 years) from a variety of countries, mainly Canada (n = 85), the USA (n = 18), and countries within Europe (n = 27) Table 3 contains additional demographic information.
Table 3
Participants Demographics
Response | n respondents |
Gender | |
Female | 78 |
Male | 54 |
Did not answer | 6 |
Country | |
Canada | 85 |
USA | 18 |
Europe | 27 |
Others | 5 |
Did not answer | 3 |
Age | |
18–19 | 3 |
20–29 | 13 |
30–39 | 29 |
40–49 | 19 |
50–59 | 26 |
60–69 | 34 |
70–76 | 10 |
VI category |
Blind | 86 |
Low vision | 43 |
Other | 6 |
Did not answer | 3 |
Age at VI onset |
0–2 | 60 |
3–11 | 17 |
12–20 | 12 |
21–50 | 23 |
50–68 | 8 |
Auditory impairment |
Yes | 24 |
No | 114 |
Type of phone |
Iphone | 120 |
Android | 23 |
Time phone use |
> 3 h/day | 78 |
1–3 h/day | 44 |
< 1h/day | 12 |
Did not answer | 3 |
Competence with technology |
Beginner | 10 |
Intermediary | 47 |
Advanced | 77 |
Did not answer | 4 |
Aids used | |
None or non-traditional aids | 20 |
White cane | 92 |
Guide dog | 26 |
Area of travel |
Urban | 105 |
Suburban | 66 |
Rural | 23 |
Frequence travel |
Everyday | 58 |
Several times a week | 54 |
Once a week | 12 |
A few times a month | 6 |
Other | 4 |
Did not answer | 3 |
Confidence in independent navigation |
Beginner | 20 |
Intermediary | 51 |
Advanced | 63 |
Did not answer | 4 |
Q1: What types of apps are used for navigation-based tasks?
In total, 125 participants completed the section about navigation-based tasks. Findings indicate that most participants (n = 120, 96%) use applications during independent travel, with only 4% (n = 5) of participants indicating that they do not use apps for navigation-based tasks at all. However, the proportions of participants using apps significantly differed between the navigation-based tasks (Q(6, n = 125) = 253.003, p < .001). More precisely, participants are less likely to use apps for street crossings and obstacle detection compared to other navigation-based tasks (p < .001). More detailed information can be found in Fig. 2 and supplementary file 2.
Apps to access visual information
Overall, the navigation-based task for which apps are most commonly adopted (76% of respondents) relates to visual interpretation (e.g., locating/identifying objects or reading environmental text). For such visual interpretation, respondents significantly preferred using specialized apps (n = 87, 91.6%) compared to mainstream apps (n = 50, 52.63%), and predominantly used computer vision/AI and human assistance apps for this purpose. For those who indicated not using apps for visual interpretation, the most common reason reported was the lack of awareness of apps to assist with this task (n = 13, 48.15%).
Apps for planning and following routes
For navigation-based tasks related to general orientation and routes (e.g., planning routes, finding POIs, using public transport, and geolocation), GPS apps were predominantly used. Mainstream apps were significantly preferred to specialized apps for planning routes (92.5% vs 43.62%) and taking public transportation (77.03% vs 52.70%), but there was no apparent preference between mainstream and specialized apps for geolocation (74.07% vs 72.84%) and finding POIs (68.60% vs 82.56%). For participants who indicated not using apps for these tasks, the main reasons identified were that respondents did not feel the need to use apps for this purpose or did not know apps that could help them.
Apps for dynamic tasks related to perceived risk
Participants were significantly less likely to use apps for dynamic navigation-based tasks (such as street crossings and obstacle detection while in movement) because most respondents: 1) already use other aids for these tasks (street crossings, n = 49; obstacle detection, n = 68); 2) don’t know apps that could help them (street crossings, n = 49; obstacle detection, n = 47); and 3) don’t feel the need to use apps for this purpose (street crossings, n = 35; obstacle detection, n = 41). Other reported reasons included the lack of trust in applications or technology in general (e.g., incomplete maps, battery life, processing delays, or smartphone processing capacities, n = 9). However, among those who do use apps for dynamic tasks (street crossings, n = 23; obstacle detection, n = 15), human assistance apps are more commonly used than those based on AI, with a preference for specialized apps over mainstream apps (street crossings, 86.95% vs 47.82%; obstacle detection, 72.22% vs 53.33%), a pattern that was only significant for street crossing.
Q2: What factors are correlated with app usage?
Among the 110 responses, 36 (32.72%) of participants indicated that they do not rely on apps for travel; 67 (60.91%) rely on apps only in unfamiliar areas, and 7 (6.36%) rely on apps for all travel. This was found to be true for both blind and low vision participants, with a significant correlation with the frequency at which participants travel. Specifically, participants who travel less often are more likely to rely on apps during travel (rho = 0.228, p = .021). Furthermore, we investigated the correlation between several factors and apps usage for the different navigation-based tasks, with the following significant correlations observed:
Age. Younger respondents were more likely to use apps for obstacle detection (rho=-0.179, p = .046) and visual identification (rho=-0.202, p = .025).
Frequency of travel. The more respondents travel, the more likely they are to use apps to prepare routes (rho = -0.263, p = .004).
Level of vision. Blind respondents were more likely to use apps for visual interpretation (X2(1, n = 116) = 12.14, p < .001), finding points of interest (X2(1, n = 116) = 5.02, p = .025), and geolocation (X2(1, n = 116) = 6.001, p = .014). In general, blind and low vision respondents equally used apps of the different categories except for visual interpretation where low-vision respondents are more likely to use image rendering apps (13/23 vs 22/68, stats), and blind respondents, to use computer vision/AI apps (66/68 vs 15/23, stats).
VI onset. Participants with earlier VI are more likely to use apps for street crossing (rho=-0.193, p = .044), finding POI (rho=-0.349, p < .001), using public transport (rho=-0.396, p < .001), and geolocation (rho=-0.282, p = .003). Similarly, participants with a longer VI duration – found to be linked with earlier VI onset (rho=-0.605, p < .001) – are also more likely to use apps for street crossing (rho = 0.323, p < .001), finding POI (rho = 0.204, p = .037), and public transport (rho = 0.254, p = .009).
Type of navigational aid. As the type of navigational aids used was closely related to level of vision (X2(2, n = 129) = 23.842, p < .001), the analysis revealed the same pattern of results. Participants using non-traditional aids, or no aids (30% of low-vision participants, 3% of blind participants) were less likely to use apps for visual interpretation (X2(2, n = 119) = 12.14, p = .002), finding POI (X2(2, n = 119) = 11.19, p = .004) and geolocation (X2(2, n = 119) = 6.02, p = .049). However, it was found that individuals using non-traditional aids, or no aids, were less likely to use apps to take public transport (X2(2, n = 119) = 8.31, p = .016).
Overall, the most reported condition that prevents the use of an app is the presence of loud ambient sounds (n = 60, 54.55%), and the least reported was high luminosity (n = 9, 8.18%). Low-vision participants are more likely to be bothered by high luminosity (16.67% vs 1.45%, X2(1, n = 105) = 8.804, p = .003). As for the factors participants take into account when selecting apps, the results show that the importance of the different factors significantly varied (Q(3,n = 110) = 121, p < .001). The most important factors were 1) the ease of use/accessibility of the app (N = 90, 81.82%); and 2) that the app responds to a certain need (n = 79, 71.82%). Moreover, the price (reported by 42 respondents, 38.18%) was perceived to be more important than the amount of data used by the app (see Fig. 3). Blind respondents were more likely to choose apps based on whether the app responds to a specific need (54/69 vs 21/36, X2(1, n = 105) = 4.603, p = .032), and also tended to choose their apps according to their ease of use/accessibility” (60/69 vs 26/36, X2(1, n = 105) = 4.5, p = .063).
Q3: Are current apps addressing navigation-based needs?
In total, 55 respondents (50%) reported having navigation-based needs that are currently not met by applications available to them. Among these, blind respondents were more likely to have unmet needs than those with low vision (38/69 vs 12/36, X2(1, n = 105) = 4.482, p = .034). Respondents were then invited to elaborate on what needs were currently not met. The 49 responses received were classified into nine categories (see Table 1 and 2*). Results showed that some categories were more frequently reported than others (Q(8, n = 49) = 41.096, p < .001). The three most reported categories of unmet needs were: “POI/geolocation” (n = 17, 34.70%), “indoor navigation” (n = 14, 28.57%), and “route planning” (N = 15, 30.61%), while the least reported categories were: “Obstacle detection” (n = 1, 2.41%), “Object Identification” (n = 2, 4.82%) and “versatility” (n = 1, 2.41%). More detailed information can be found in Fig. 3 and supplementary file 2. Additionally, respondents traveling in semi-urban/suburbs were more likely to have unmet needs for POI/geolocation, than those not traveling in such areas (11/19 vs 6/30, X2(1, N = 49) = 7.373, p = .007).