This study examines dysfunctional mothering characterized by child physical neglect to explore convergence within the methylation profiles of both mothers and their children, covering the entire methylation array and specific genes. We also investigated how psychological covariables modulate the convergence in the mother-child methylation. In contrast to the standard approach employed, where covariation between two individuals is established through averaged methylation values in a gene or a set of CpGs, as observed in [17], our study adopted a novel strategy. We correlated the methylation profiles of mother and child while preserving the sequence of methylation values across CpGs for each individual rather than the average. This approach draws inspiration from methodologies used to compare methylation profiles between distinct human tissues within the same individual, such as peripheral blood and brain tissues [44-46].
We first examined whether there was a higher MSI in mother-child familial pairs than in random pairs. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that in the entire epigenome, the mean of the similarity indexes was significantly higher in familial pairs with a genetic and environmental connection, than in randomly paired mothers and children. Furthermore, this finding was consistently supported when analyzing stress-regulation genes such as NR3C1, FKBP5, OXTR, and BDNF, although not in the case of the serotonin transporter SLC6A4 gen.
For our second objective, we examined the convergence of methylation profiles between mothers and children, both at the epigenome level and within specific genes. This convergence was significant only in the familial pairs and linked to maternal exposure to adverse events, child age as indicator of the time of co-residence, and neglect and control groups. The combined use of these covariates may shed more light on whether the observed mother-child transmission patterns are linked to the inheritance of epigenetic stress-related events or their shared environment [47]. Concerning the mothers’ DNAm effects from their exposure to adverse events, our results point out opposite intergenerational effects on their children's DNAm. At the epigenome level, we found that the higher the mother’s life stress, the lower the MSI between mothers and their children, no matter their sex.
This epigenome-wide opposite mother-child trend is also evident in two specific genes, BDNF and OXTR, where a reduced MSI was also found in presence of the covariate mother’s exposure to adverse life events. Another study also found an inversed pattern in methylation in the same site in a functional intronic region of stress-related gene FKBP5 in parents and offspring associated with preconceived maternal stress [15]. Regarding the OXTR receptor, our study revealed that neglected children had a lower MSI with their mothers compared to those children in control dyads. Typically, mothers who engage in neglectful caregiving often have experienced more life adversities in their own life [48]. Therefore, it is likely that they showed less epigenetic convergence with their children. A similar pattern was observed in one study with the oxytocin receptor gene [49]. This study found that mean OXTR methylation in mothers and newborns was positively associated with dyads in which mothers did not have experienced childhood maltreatment, but not in dyads in which mothers did.
A potential explanation of our results is that exposure to growing adversity increases methylation variability with a higher impact on the mother than on the child, probably due to her longer time of stress exposure. In that sense, our mothers may have more methylation changes than normative mothers increasing the epigenetic distance with their children under the effect of stressful life events. On a more theoretical grounds, the directional distinction observed between methylation profiles in the mothers and their children may be indicative of an adaptive response by offspring during the child's sensitive developmental period, aimed at mitigating the biological impacts of maternal exposure to trauma [15, 49]. Nevertheless, given that preconception and postnatal influences are both possible, further research involving mother-child dyads is necessary to delineate the conditions under which methylation patterns exhibit either negative or positive correlations with maternal adversity across generations. This will allow for a more comprehensive evaluation of our inverse trend.
A pattern of increasing mother-child MSI was obtained for the length of time that the child has lived with the mother (co-residence time), signaling the relevance of the mother-child shared environment. Interestingly, an early effect of epigenetic similarity was found from maternal-umbilical cord blood in certain promotor regions and in highly repeated elements, such as long interspersed nucleotide elements (LINE-1) and Alu, which may serve as surrogate markers for global DNA methylation [47]. Epigenetic coincidence has also been found in the context of post-traumatic stress in maternal blood, placental tissue, and umbilical cord blood in gen BDNF [50]. Building upon this initial similarity, our study has evidenced that continued mother-child residence facilitates a greater degree of methylation similarity, by exposing both members of the dyad to similar environmental factors. This epigenetic similarity becomes more pronounced as the duration of time shared together increases, as indicated by the child's age.
The co-residential effect on the within-dyad epigenetic similarity has also been demonstrated in different samples of monozygotic twins. In one study, the sample of twins was divided into two age groups, below and above 18 years old, differentiating twins living together from those living apart [51]. They found less epigenetic similarity in the group of older twins living apart, attributed consequently to the environmental differences or stochastic factors since the genetic component was automatically controlled by design. Other studies also found that correlations in methylation levels increased with the time twins lived together [26, 52]. Notably, no differences were found between monozygotic and dizygotic twins, supporting the relevance of the shared environment for greater epigenetic convergence rather than the genetic factor of being monozygotic. It was also found that epigenetic similarity decays (equally for monozygotic and dizygotic) with the time the twins lived apart. Although we did not control for another group of dyads living separately or considered genetic factors as a separate variable, our results go in the same direction since time of living together is a factor affecting the epigenetic convergence between mother and child.
Finally, a brief note is deserved to the empathy effect, in the direction that greater Mother empathy increased the mother-child MSI for the oxytocin receptor gene OXTR. Although this trend did not survive multiple comparison corrections, it theoretically underscores the relevance of empathic care since OXTR has been implicated in a range of early social behaviors related to bonding and attachment relationships [28, 29]. Maternal empathic care tentatively appears to be involved in the OXTR regulation of developmental experiences displayed during early mother-child interactions. These positive exchanges have been shown to play a critical role in establishing secure child attachment and ensuring subsequent health and well-being [53, 54].
Despite being the only epigenetic similarity study with well-characterized mother-child dyads in the neglect condition and evaluated in tandem, our study has limitations. Additional explanatory data of the biological transmission, such as genetic variants (Single Nucleotide Polymorphism) affecting the differentially methylated CpGs, were not assessed due to a lack of sample genotyping. Difficulty in finding neglectful mother-child dyads limited our sample size to 115 mother-child dyads, which is relatively small in epigenetic studies but is larger than the average size of the dyadic studies cited (mean = 102 dyads) [15-17, 47, 55]. Nevertheless, future studies are required to replicate our findings with larger sample sizes. However, the significant results achieved through epigenome-wide analysis and rigorous methodological controls bolster our confidence that the sample size has not increased in Type 2 errors. Furthermore, our innovative methodology, which focused on the mother-child methylation profile similarity as the dependent variable rather than individual methylation averages, offers a more comprehensive way of mapping mother-child epigenetic coincidences.
In conclusion, the analysis of the dyadic epigenetic similarity in the methylation profiles has allowed us to confirm the greater methylation correlation in familial pairs than in random pairs both epigenome-wide and in stress-relevant genes NR3C1, FKBP5, OXTR and BDNF. Moreover, using different covariates as modulating factors in the intergenerational methylation process offers a valuable opportunity to gain insights into development-dependent adaptations that are influenced by both hereditary and environmental factors, significantly observed only in biological dyads. Two opposing driving forces have been identified influencing the epigenome array and genes BDNF and OXTR: one leading to reduced epigenetic convergence, and the other supporting a greater convergence. Mother adversity and belonging to the neglect group lead to reduced epigenetic convergence revealing a lower potential of such risk factors for mother-to-child methylation transmission. By contrast, mother-child time of co-residence, indexed by child age, promotes increased epigenetic convergence, evidencing a higher potential for methylation transmission during the ontogeny. The same convergence tendency, although not reached a significant level, showed the mother’s empathic concern trait in the OXTR gene.
Our findings have twofold implications for child well-being. On the positive side, children of mothers exposed to life adversity or neglect did not necessarily inherit a direct replica of their methylation patterns. The other is worrisome, since living with their mother in a negligent context, is a crucial environmental factor with a high impact of epigenetic transmission on children, reinforcing the need for “the earlier, the better” recommendation of the Child Protection System, which is not always followed. In response to this alarm, it is urgent to prevent the progressive epigenetic impact of chronic and unnoticed situations of child neglect resulting from longer shared exposure to an insensitive and unstimulating immediate environment. To this aim, it is important to expand the possibilities for an early diagnosis of the neglect condition in both the mother, who has experienced trauma, and her newborn child, being performed at primary care screenings. Next, for those cases with early signs of neglect risk, training in mother-child stimulation and empathic care should be incorporated into targeted interventions to break the cycle of intergenerational transmission of child neglect and prevent subsequent negative outcomes.