This systematic review aims to investigate the role of emotions in the academic performance of undergraduate medical students. Because of the heterogeneity of the data collection tools and different research designs, meta-analysis cannot be used (17). Therefore, narrative synthesis was adopted in this chapter. The studies are grouped into four categories as follows: 1) The effect of depression and anxiety on academic performance, 2) Test anxiety and academic achievement, 3) Shame and academic performance, and 4) Academic emotions and medical students. The control-value theory (18) will be used to interpret the findings.
The effect of depression and anxiety on academic performance
According to the retrieved research, depression and anxiety can have both a negative and a positive impact on the academic performance of medical students. Severe anxiety may impair memory function, decrease concentration, lead to a state of hypervigilance, interfere with judgment and cognitive function, and further affect academic performance (3). Most of the retrieved articles found that anxiety and depression were associated with low academic performance (2, 8, 11, 12). Moreira (2018) and Mihailescu (2016) found that higher depression levels were associated with more failed courses and a lower GPA, but they did not find any association between anxiety level and academic performance.
By contrast, some studies have suggested that experiencing some level of anxiety reinforces students’ motivation to improve their academic performance (8, 19). Zalihic et al. (2017) conducted a study to investigate anxiety sensitivity about academic success and noticed a positive relationship between anxiety level and high academic scores; they justified this because, when medical students feel anxious, they tend to prepare and study more, and they desire to achieve better scores and fulfil social expectations. Similarly, Jamil et al study (2022) found anxiety has a negative impact on academic performance when excessive and a positive effect when manageable, in which case it encourages medical students and motivates them to achieve higher scores.
In the wider literature, the impact of anxiety on academic performance has contradictory research findings. While some studies suggest that having some level of anxiety can boost students' motivation to improve their academic performance, other research has shown that anxiety has a negative impact on their academic success (20, 21).
Test Anxiety and Academic Achievement
The majority of the studies reviewed confirm that test anxiety negatively affects academic performance (13–15). Several research have found a significant correlation between test anxiety and academic achievement, indicating that higher levels of test anxiety are associated with lower exam scores and lower academic performance (22, 23). For example, Green et al. (2016) RCT study found that test anxiety has a moderately significant negative correlation with USMLE score. They found that medical students who took the test-taking strategy course had lower levels of test anxiety than the control group, and their test anxiety scores after the exam had improved from the baseline. Although their test anxiety improved after taking the course, there was no significant difference in the exam scores between students who had and had not taken the course. Therefore, the intervention they used was not effective. According to the control-value theory, this intervention can be improved if they design an emotionally effective learning environment, clear instructional design, foster self-regulation of negative emotions, and teach students emotion-oriented regulation (18).
Additionally, according to this theory, students who perceive exams as difficult are more likely to experience test anxiety because test anxiety results from a student’s negative appraisal of the task and outcome values, leading to a reduction in their performance. This aligns with Kim’s (2016) study, which found that students who believed that the OSCE was a difficult exam experienced test anxiety more than other students (18, 24).
In the wider literature, the British Psychology Society (2022) reported that higher test anxiety was associated with poorer performance on a final exam. They conducted a longitudinal study on students' knowledge levels during a semester before the exam and discovered that test anxiety was associated with lower performance on mock exams. Moreover, Song et al. (2021) experimental study examined the effects of test anxiety on working memory capacity and found that test anxiety negatively predicts academic performance. Therefore, the evidence from Song’s study suggests that there is a small but significant size effect of anxiety on working memory capacity. However, another cross-sectional study revealed that test anxiety in medical students had no significant effect on exam performance (25). The complexities of this relationship necessitate additional investigation. Since the retrieved articles are from different countries, it is critical to recognise the possible impact of cultural differences on the impact of test anxiety. Cultural factors such as different educational systems, assessment tools and societal expectations may lead to variances in test anxiety experience and expression across diverse communities (26, 27).
Shame and academic performance
The systematic review examined three studies that discuss the impact of feelings of shame on academic performance (2, 9, 10). Generally, shame is considered a negative emotion which involves self-reflection and self-evaluation, and it leads to rumination and self-condemnation (28). Intimate examinations conducted by medical students can induce feelings of shame, affecting their ability to communicate with patients and their clinical decisions. Shame can increase the avoidance of intimate physical examinations and also encourage clinical practice (2, 9, 10).
One study found that some medical students felt shame during simulations-based education examinations because they had made incorrect decisions, which decreased their self-esteem and motivation to learn. However, others who felt shame were motivated to study harder to avoid repeating the same mistakes (10). Shame was decreased with more clinical practice, but shame did not affect their learning or performance (9). The existing literature regarding how shame affects medical students’ learning is inconclusive (29).
In the wider literature, shame is considered maladaptive, leading to dysfunctional behaviour, encouraging withdrawal and avoidance of events and inhibiting social interaction, but few studies have been conducted on shame in the medical field. Therefore, more research is needed to investigate the role of shame in medical students’ academic performance (28).
Academic emotions and medical students.
The literature review focused on three studies that examined the relationship between academic emotions and the academic achievements of medical students (2, 10, 16). Academic emotions refer to the emotions that are associated with academic outcomes(16).
Behren's (2019) mixed-method study on the achievement emotions of medical students during simulations, found that placing students in challenging clinical cases that they can handle raises positive emotions. These emotions were perceived by students as a positive drive for learning, while mild anxiety was considered beneficial for learning. However, the study also found non-significant correlations between emotions and performance during the simulation, indicating a complex relationship between emotions and academic performance. The results revealed that feelings of frustration were perceived to reduce students' interest and motivation for studying, hampered their decision-making process, and negatively affected their self-esteem, which is consistent with the academic achievement emotions literature where negative emotions are associated with poor intrinsic motivation and reduced the ability to learn (30).
The reviewed research reveals a positive correlation between positive emotions and academic performance and a negative correlation between negative emotions and academic performance. These findings align with the control–value theory (18), which suggests that positive emotions facilitate learning through mediating factors including cognitive learning strategies such as strategic thinking, critical thinking and problem-solving and metacognitive learning strategies such as monitoring, regulating, and planning and students’ intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Additionally, several studies found that extrinsic motivation from the educational environment and the application of cognitive and emotional strategies improve students’ ability to learn and consequently their academic performance (2, 10, 16). By contrast, negative emotions negatively affect academic performance. This is because negative emotions reduce students’ motivation, ability to focus, and ability to process information (2, 10, 16).
Limitations of the study
This systematic review aims to thoroughly investigate the relationship between emotions and academic performance in undergraduate medical students, but it has inherent limitations. Overall, the methodological quality of the retrieved studies is mostly good and fair. They demonstrated strengths in sampling techniques and data collection. However, other drawbacks were discovered, most notably the need for a more causal relationship, which is inherent in the design of cross-sectional studies. Furthermore, given the reliance on self-reported data, there were concerns about potential recall bias. These methodological difficulties were noted in all of the examined research. When contemplating the implications for practice and future study, the impact of these limitations on the validity of the data should be acknowledged.
The limitation of the review process and the inclusion criteria restricted the study to articles published from January 2013 to December 2023, potentially overlooking relevant research conducted beyond this timeframe. Additionally, the exclusive focus on undergraduate medical students may constrain the applicability of findings to other health fields or educational levels.
Moreover, the exclusion of non-English language articles and those not published in peer-reviewed journals introduces potential language and publication biases. The reliance on electronic databases and specific keywords may inadvertently omit studies using different terms or indexing. While the search strategy is meticulous, it might not cover every relevant study due to variations in indexing and database coverage. However, the involvement of three assessors for study screening, selection, data extraction and quality assessment improved the robustness of the review and ensured that the review included all the relevant research.
In conclusion, these limitations highlight the need for careful interpretation of the study's findings and stress the importance of future research addressing these constraints to offer a more comprehensive understanding of the nuanced relationship between emotions and academic performance in undergraduate medical education.