Accuracy in 2-interval forced-choice decisions
We began the analyses with examining the ability of the participants to discriminate in which one of the two intervals the target appeared. Accuracy of 2IFC responses in the masked condition did not differ statistically significantly between orientation (M = .80, SD = .11) and color (M = .81, SD = .09) discrimination tasks, t(25) = -0.48, p = .637, d = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.06, 0.04]. Neither was there any difference between orientation and color conditions in the control trials in 2IFC decision accuracy: the performance was at ceiling for both orientation (M = .99, SD = .02) and color (M = .99, SD = .04). One of the participants made one error in the control trials in the orientation condition, and three participants made one error in the color condition. The high performance level in the control trials confirms that the participants were following the instructions and were able to detect the target when the target-mask SOA was long.
Discrimination of orientation and color
Next, we studied the accuracy rates in orientation and color discrimination in masked trials with generalized linear mixed effects model on single trials. Figure 2A presents the observed results and Fig. 2B the modelled results. The overall model predicting discrimination accuracy with Feature and the accuracy in 2IFC responses (accuracy ~ Feature * 2IFC + (2IFC | participant)) had an explanatory power (conditional R2) of 2.72%, in which he fixed effects' part was 3.81% (marginal R2). The model's intercept was at 0.41 (SE = 0.13, p = .002, 95% CI [0.15, 0.68]), indicating that orientation was discriminated better than chance in trials where the interval was discriminated incorrectly (i.e., the participant decided that the target appeared in the other interval). Discrimination of color did not differ significantly from that of orientation in incorrect 2IFC trials (beta = 0.07, SE = 0.15, 95% CI [-0.22, 0.36], z = 0.49, p = .627). Orientation discrimination in correct 2IFC trials succeeded better than in incorrect ones (beta = 0.73, SE = 0.14, 95% CI [0.45, 1.02], z = 5.10, p < .001), and the lack of Feature x 2IFC interaction (beta = 0.26, SE = 0.17, 95% CI [-0.073, 0.60], z = 1.54, p = .124) suggests that color was, similarly as compared with orientation, discriminated better when the 2IFC decision was correct.
As non-planned control analysis, we run the previous analysis with excluding the four participants who made errors in 2IFC responses in control trials. The results (n = 22) replicated the finding that the intercept was higher than expected by chance (beta = 0.50, SE = 0.15, z = 3.283, p = 0.001, 95% CI [0.20, 0.81]), which means that in trials with incorrect 2IFC responses the probability of making correct orientation discrimination was higher than that of making an incorrect orientation discrimination. The color discrimination did not differ from that of orientation (beta = 0.035, SE = 0.16, z = 0.21, p = 0.831, 95% CI [-0.29, 0.36]. Performance was better in correct 2IFC responses than in incorrect ones (beta = 0.80, SE = 0.16, z = 5.05, p < .001, 95% CI [0.48, 1.12], and this effect did not interact with feature (beta = 0.04, 0.19, z = 0.226, p = .821, 95% CI [-0.33, 0.41]. Thus, the participants who performed perfectly the 2IFC choices in control trials showed in masked conditions above chance level performance in discriminating the features even when the 2IFC response was incorrect.
Because of low number of 2IFC errors in the control trials, the discrimination accuracy was analysed only for the control trials in which the 2IFC decision was made correctly. Figure 3A shows the observed discrimination performance in control trials, whereas Fig. 3B displays the modelled results. For the controls trials, the overall model predicting discrimination accuracy (accuracy ~ Feature + (1|id)) had an explanatory power (conditional R2) of 7.50%, in which the fixed effects' part was 20.87% (marginal R2). The model's intercept was at 2.95 (SE = 0.55, 95% CI [2.04, 4.42]). Within this model, the effect of Feature was significant (beta = 2.52, SE = 0.68, 95% CI [1.34, 4.07], z = 3.73, p < .001), indicating that color was discriminated better than orientation. However, Fig. 3A shows that for both features most of the participants performed at ceiling.
Comparison of accuracy between feature discrimination and 2IFC tasks
Finally, we compared accuracy of discriminating the feature (orientation or color) in all masked trials, without differentiating correct and incorrect 2IFC decisions, and accuracy in the 2IFC decisions in all masked trials (Fig. 4). A repeated-measures analysis of variance with Feature and Task (discrimination vs. 2IFC) as variables showed that the effect of Task was significant, F(1, 96) = 8.20, p = .005, partial omega-squared = 0.07, showing that accuracy in the 2IFC task (80%) was higher than in the feature discrimination task (74%). The effect of Feature (F(1, 96) = 1.70, p = .195, partial omega-squared = 0.01), and the interaction between Feature and Task (F(1, 96) = 1.12, p = .293, partial omega-squared = 0.001) were not statistically significant. These analyses suggest that the 2IFC task was easier than the feature discrimination task.