In the current study, we sought to investigate the phenotypic similarities and differences between online participants with high self-reported autism traits and those with an ASD diagnosis confirmed in-person via clinician evaluation. We identified a lack of agreement between self-rated and clinician-assessed symptom measures, highlighting the need for separate interpretations of each. When investigating each group’s social behavior, we found that individuals with confirmed ASD showed impairments in recognizing opportunities to exert social control and reduced affiliation in their interactions with virtual characters; in contrast, high-trait individuals identified online showed comparable social behaviors to low-trait individuals. These results provide a caution for future online research: when attempting to identify and draw conclusions about certain diagnostic groups, self-report alone may not be sufficient.
Despite the overall lack of measurement agreement identified in this study, we do not believe that these results suggest that self-report questionnaires are invalid for ASD research. On the contrary, they are important tools for understanding individual’s subjective experiences and levels of internal distress or wellbeing. Self-reports are also critical for ensuring individuals with lived experience have a role in shaping the narrative surrounding them, and can help challenge baseless assumptions regarding the intentions or reasoning behind autistic behavior. Rather than dismiss the importance of subjective self-views, the results provide a caution for the use of self-report alone for defining or extrapolating about a diagnostic group.
We saw no relationship between self-reported BAPQ and clinician-rated ADOS symptom scores in the in-person ASD group, consistent with previous reports using different measures9–11. Discrepancies between self- and observer-rated symptoms are not uncommon amongst individuals with altered introspection; they have been reported in a variety of conditions characterized by impaired insight, including depression28 and schizophrenia29. Evidence suggests that insight difficulties in such conditions may be more pronounced in certain domains. Among individuals with schizophrenia, for instance, those with reduced insight have been shown to over-report their levels of extroversion but accurately reported other personality traits, suggesting insight may play a significant role in the reporting of social tendencies specifically30. Reduced social self-insight has been widely reported in ASD31,32 and likely contributes to discrepancies between self- and clinician-report. It is possible that, despite presenting with relatively normative social behavior to the outside observer, the ASD individuals with higher social awareness report experiencing more social difficulties due to increased insight into their social limitations and differences from typically developing peers31,33.
In the social controllability task, the ASD group rejected a smaller percentage of high offers in the controllable condition compared to the online groups. This reduced rejection of “good” offers hindered their ability to receive better offers down the line, suggesting they did not take advantage of the controllability offered by the condition. In line with this, we also saw that the ASD group did not self-report any differences in the perceived controllability of the conditions. Such results may stem from reductions in ToM-related understanding of others’ motivations in the clinical ASD group but not the high-trait group. To distinguish between random and non-random behaviors on the part of the players, one must realize that they are motivated to receive the largest amount of money possible. To achieve this understanding, you might use prior information (i.e., past offers) to build expectations about future behaviors (i.e., players will give you repeatedly low offers as long as you continue to accept them) that would fit a given intention (i.e., players want to maximize gain) and evaluate their accuracy. In ASD, impaired ability to predict offers and understand players’ intentions may lead to a lack of distinction between random and non-random (goal-directed) behavior. Indeed, individuals with ASD display reduced understanding of social intentions, including whether actions are goal-directed34, that appears to stem from impaired use of prior social information to form expectations35. It’s also possible that the reduced perception of controllability seen in ASD is caused by impaired affordance perception, which refers to the ability to ascertain which actions are available for you to take in a given environment. Autistic individuals have been shown to inaccurately estimate action capabilities in the perceptual-motor domain36, and such impairments are theorized to extend into the social domain37. In any case, the high-trait and low-trait online groups showed comparable behavior across all task measures, suggesting that this impaired detection of others’ goal-directed behaviors and/or perception of the actions available to oneself is specific to individuals with a confirmed ASD diagnosis.
In the social navigation task, though both the high-trait and ASD groups reported liking the characters less than the low-trait group, only the ASD group was less affiliative with characters during their interactions than other groups. Such results highlight the importance of measuring behavior for achieving a comprehensive understanding of symptom presentation. The high-trait and ASD groups were aligned in their subjective beliefs, both about their symptoms and their opinions of others, but these beliefs did not translate into comparable social tendencies. Considering that pro-affiliative behavior is often considered to be polite, and that individuals with ASD frequently exhibit diminished adherence to social conventions38, this difference may be reflective of reduced awareness of or desire to follow friendliness norms in ASD. In contrast, those without a confirmed diagnosis may be more inclined or better able to act friendly despite their internal discomfort and dislike of characters. In line with this idea, though reduced character liking was associated with increased self-reported symptoms in all groups, we only detected a relationship between self-reported symptoms and affiliative behavior within the ASD group – those with a higher level of symptoms were the least friendly with the characters. Such results provide further evidence that self-reported symptoms have difference implications in individuals with and without a confirmed ASD diagnosis. Altogether, the findings from both tasks suggest that samples defined by online self-report are phenotypically distinct from clinically-ascertained samples, and that using such online samples to answer questions about social interaction may not be informative about ASD as a whole.
In our study, the online group with high autistic traits also self-reported heightened levels of social anxiety and avoidant personality disorder symptoms compared to the in-person ASD group. This difference suggests that self-reported autism symptoms in the general population may be more reflective of general social avoidance and self-consciousness regarding social skills rather than autism-specific social difficulties. Supporting the existence of this phenotype, largescale online studies investigating latent psychiatric factors in the general population have identified transdiagnostic dimensions characterized by similar socially-avoidant/anxious traits39,24. As we have shown, these online participants who report elevated internal social difficulties (i.e., emotional or cognitive struggles that others may not notice, as described by self-report) also show different social behaviors from those with a clinical diagnosis who show elevated external difficulties (i.e., inappropriate actions or visible struggles, as described by clinician-report), suggesting the diagnosis and the dimension are not synonymous. Though ASD is highly comorbid with social anxiety, it is still only represented in less than half of cases40, and comorbidity with avoidant personality disorder is even less common41. It may be the case that self-reported internal symptoms lack diagnostic specificity, especially at subclinical thresholds, whereas clinicians are able to better assign clinically-significant symptoms to separate diagnoses through observing external behaviors.
An important implication of this distinction is that we must be cautious not to extrapolate about the needs of one group based on the findings from research conducted in the other. For example, the individuals with high self-reported autism, anxiety, and avoidance traits, despite doing reasonably well by external metrics of social abilities, may need intervention towards boosting self-confidence, and reducing anxiety and negative self-talk rather than social skills trainings. In contrast, the individuals who self-report few symptoms but present to clinicians with observable difficulties in social interaction may benefit from more skills-focused training to aid in quality-of-life outcomes like independent living, relationships, and employment. This distinction is important because it presents a potential risk of harm (or at least reduced access to benefits) to autistic individuals who require more behavioral support and their access to accommodations; If online self-report-based samples are used to represent the whole diagnostic spectrum despite clear differences in behavior, the implications for intervention may be biased.
This study should be interpreted with the following limitations in mind. First, we relied on a single self-reported autism symptom measure - the BAPQ - because of its strong psychometric properties in both the general population and in those with an ASD diagnosis15,42,43. However, other surveys such as the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ)44 are also commonly used in research to assess autistic traits and do not always converge with clinical/caregiver impressions9,12,14, similar to the BAPQ-ADOS discrepancy identified in the current study. Second, since the inception of this study, Prolific has added a screening tool that allows researchers to specifically select participants that self-report having received a formal clinical diagnosis of ASD. However, this information is still self-reported and unverifiable. Future work should investigate if the use of additional symptom measures and/or self-reported diagnoses in online studies would identify a group that shows behavior more closely aligned with the ASD phenotype. Lastly, we do not have evidence to examine if the current findings are specific to ASD or generalizable to other psychiatric diagnoses such as schizophrenia or personality disorders where impaired insight can be a symptom. Future research is needed to investigate the broader implications of this work.
As online research continues to proliferate, we must consider the limitations of online approaches when determining which scientific questions they are best suited to answer. Questions about transdiagnostic traits and symptoms, for example, avoid the issues with diagnostic specificity in self-report and may be well-suited for testing with online platforms, especially for traits not associated with impaired insight. Online research is a powerful tool that will continue to help answer important questions in human-subject research. However, the results of the current study suggest that online approaches in psychiatry should be used in tandem with, rather than as a replacement for, lab-based research, and that over-generalization of findings should be avoided in research relying on self-reported symptoms. For questions that require big-data, researchers have other tools at their disposal: pooling resources, developing cross-site collaborations, or utilizing resources like Simons Foundation Powering Autism Research (SPARK)45 will allow for large-scale replications of lab-based studies in ASD that are less reliant on self-report.