Innate adaptive defensive learning: mice adjust behavioral response to a potential threat.
We implemented the VLS paradigm to investigate an animal’s capacity to adjust innate defensive behaviors in the absence of a real risk (Fig. 1a). Mice were placed in a plexiglass apparatus that contained a rectangular nest in one corner and a projector screen that displayed a VLS while animals actively explored the arena. The detection of a VLS from above, but not from a side view (Supplementary Fig. 1a-c), triggered an immediate defensive response of freezing followed by running to the nest (here defined as early responses to VLS). Animals also spent a significant amount of time in the confines of the shelter, presumably avoiding potential threat, before engaging in exploratory behaviors (here defined as late responses to VLS). However, with multiple exposures of an overhead VLS for 3 consecutive days, without evidence of aversive outcomes, mice learned to adjust both early and late VLS-evoked innate defensive strategies (Fig. 1b-j). Across the 3 days, immediate freezing significantly reduced (Fig. 1c-d), whereas speed increased (Fig. 1e-f). Mice continued to run with similar maximum speed (10 sec upon VLS) and exhibited similar escape latencies to the nest in response to repeated VLS over the 3 days (Supplementary Fig. 1d-e). To measure late VLS-induced defensive behaviors, we tracked the animal’s position throughout arena zones (Supplementary Fig. 1f and methods). The amount of time spent inside the nest upon VLS exposure decreased over repeated days (Fig. 1g-h), while animals shifted to more exploratory behavior near the walls (Fig. 1i-j). We did not observe adaptive changes with time spent in the trigger zone, where the VLS is presented (Supplementary Fig. 1g-h), or time in the safety zone adjacent to the nest (Supplementary Fig. 1i-j). Notably, adjustment of defensive responses was not detected within a single-day trial session (Supplementary Fig. 2), suggesting that optimization of defensive strategies may reflect learning and consolidation processes.
We performed linear correlation analysis to further investigate threat-evoked innate adaptive defensive learning with repeated VLS (Supplementary Fig. 3 and Supplementary Tables 1–6). For early responses to VLS, we found that latency to the nest showed a strong negative correlation with the maximum speed the animals reached 10 sec upon VLS, which was maintained across the 3 sessions, indicating the faster the animals ran, the earlier they entered the nest. Interestingly, the early response of freezing upon VLS initiation predicted late defensive responses such as the total amount of time the animals would spend inside the nest. Time spent in the nest was also positively correlated with escape including latency to the nest and maximum speed 10 sec upon VLS only on day 1, but not in later sessions. These results suggest that escape behaviors (i.e. running to the nest) and avoidance behaviors (i.e. time spent inside the nest) are related to each other at initial sessions but may become more dissociable once the animals learn to adjust defensive responses.
Exposure to potential threat engages activity of IPN GAD2 neurons that adjusts with defensive learning.
To study the neurocircuitry behind threat adaptation and defensive learning we focused on the IPN of the midbrain, an emerging region associated with anxiety and fear (31). The IPN is an inhibitory nucleus highly enriched in GABAergic neurons that respond to aversive stimuli (32–34). We combined in vivo fiber photometry recordings with mouse behavior to test if IPN GABAergic neuronal activity is engaged by VLS. Specifically, we expressed Cre-dependent GCaMP in the IPN of mice driving Cre recombinase under the control of the glutamic acid decarboxylase 2 enzyme promoter (GAD2Cre mice) and recorded IPN activity dynamics time-locked to VLS events (Fig. 2a). In mice presented with an overhead VLS, we detected a significant increase in IPN GAD2 neuronal activity that was absent in control mice expressing Cre-dependent eGFP in the IPN instead of GCaMP or if the same VLS was presented from a side view (Supplementary Fig. 4a-b). Additional aversive stimuli, including a tail lift or a foot shock, also raised the activity of the IPN GAD2 neuronal population (Supplementary Fig. 4c-d). Remarkably, IPN neuronal responses to the VLS decreased from day 1 to day 3, as mice learned to optimize innate defensive strategies (Fig. 2b-e). Other behaviors, such as rearing against the wall, also increased IPN activity. However, the IPN GABAergic activity during rearing remained stable across the 3 days (Supplementary Fig. 4e), providing evidence that the decrease in signal was not due to photobleaching. Reductions of VLS-evoked IPN GAD2 neuronal activation across days were not detected within a trial session (Supplementary Fig. 4f-g).
Interestingly, IPN GAD2 neuronal activity dynamics inversely mirrored changes in speed during VLS events (Fig. 2b), as well as across the whole VLS session (Supplementary Fig. 5a). Correlation analysis demonstrated VLS-induced activation levels of IPN GAD2 neurons negatively related with speed 10 sec post-VLS initiation (Supplementary Fig. 5b). Furthermore, VLS-induced engagement of IPN GAD2 neurons positively correlated with latency to nest (Supplementary Fig. 5c) and time spent near the walls (Supplementary Fig. 5d). Notably, we also detected reduced activity of IPN GABAergic neurons when mice entered the nest (Fig. 2f-h), although these signals did not adjust across repeated sessions. Altogether these results suggested the dynamics of IPN GAD2 neuronal activity reflected different aspects of VLS evoked defensive actions.
Silencing activity of IPN GAD2 neurons during VLS presentations reduces innate defensive behaviors.
To determine the functional implication of IPN GAD2 neurons in defensive responses, we used optogenetics and selectively silenced these neurons during VLS events. GAD2Cre mice were injected with Cre-dependent halorhodopsin (NpHR) or eGFP control virus and implanted with an optic fiber in the IPN (Fig. 3a). Animals then underwent the 3-day VLS paradigm with photoinhibition 2 sec prior to VLS, which remained ON until 2 sec post-VLS, on days 1 and 3 (Fig. 3b). Photoinhibition of IPN GABAergic neurons reduced both early and late defensive responses. Compared to the controls, IPN NpHR animals exhibited a decrease in VLS-induced immediate freezing response (Fig. 3c-d) along with increased speed upon VLS display (Fig. 3e-f). Additionally, silencing IPN GAD2 neurons led to decreased time spent in the nest on day 1 of the looming session (Fig. 3g-h); instead, animals remained in the vicinity of the safety zone (Supplementary Fig. 6a-b). Other defensive responses including latency to enter the nest (Supplementary Fig. 6c), time spent near the walls (Supplementary Fig. 6d-e), or in the trigger area (Supplementary Fig. 6f-g) were not significantly affected by IPN GAD2 photoinhibition. Overall, these data suggest that IPN GABAergic neurons mediate both early and late VLS-evoked defensive behaviors, particularly on the first exposure session when neurons are highly engaged by VLS.
To complement these experiments, we also asked if activating IPN GABAergic neurons is enough to elicit changes in innate defensive strategies. To this aim, we injected the IPN of GAD2Cre mice with Cre-dependent channelrhodopsin (ChR2) and implanted an optic fiber in the target site (Supplementary Fig. 7a). In this experiment, no VLS was presented; instead, animals received IPN GAD2 neuronal photostimulation when they entered the trigger zone, on days 1 and 3 (Supplementary Fig. 7b). Optogenetic excitation alone did not produce differences between groups for the defensive responses measured (Supplementary Fig. 7c-k).
IPN GAD2 neurons projecting to the LDTg are engaged by VLS and reduce activation with innate defensive adaptive learning.
The IPN GAD2 neuronal population consists of projection neurons that innervate brain regions involved in affective and motivational behaviors, including the raphe and the LDTg (26, 27). Here, we hypothesize the IPN may use the LDTg, an area previously associated with fear and anxiety (35), to convey innate defensive behaviors. To test if this circuit responded to the VLS, we recorded activity dynamics in GAD2Cre mice bilaterally injected with a retrogradely-transported AAVrg Cre-dependent GCaMP into the LDTg and implanted with an optic fiber in the IPN (Fig. 4a). The presentation of overhead VLS triggered an elevated activation of GAD2IPN◊LDTg neurons that significantly decreased over consecutive sessions (Fig. 4b-e). No reductions in VLS-induced GAD2IPN◊LDTg circuit engagement were detected within a trial session (Supplementary Fig. 8a-b). Similarly to overall IPN GAD2 neurons, activity dynamics of the GAD2IPN◊LDTg circuit inversely mirrored changes in speed (Fig. 4b) and negatively correlated with speed levels 10 sec upon VLS initiation (Supplementary Fig. 8c). Additional analysis did not find significant correlations between VLS-induced GAD2IPN◊LDTg circuit engagement and latency to nest (Supplementary Fig. 8d), although GAD2IPN◊LDTg activation positively predicted the time mice spent near the walls (Supplementary Fig. 8e). Akin to IPN GAD2 measures, GAD2IPN◊LDTg neurons showed a decrease in circuit activity when animals entered inside the nest that was sustained across the three sessions (Fig. 4f-h). This data demonstrates that VLS-related information engages the projection from the IPN to the LDTg area, which may participate in threat processing and adaptive defensive learning.
Silencing IPN GAD2 neurons projecting to the LDTg occludes innate defensive adaptive learning.
To test if IPN GABAergic IPN neurons projecting to the LDTg are necessary for innate adaptive defensive responses, we used closed-loop optogenetic approaches in a circuit-specific manner. To this aim, we injected a retrograde Cre-dependent AAVrg NpHR or eGFP into the LDTg and placed an optic fiber in the IPN (Fig. 5a). Animals underwent the same VLS paradigm with circuit photoinhibition time-locked to VLS events. Experimental and control mice demonstrated a significant reduction in the early defensive behavior of freezing across days (Fig. 5b-c). However, compared to the controls, optogenetic silencing of the IPN◊LDTg circuit slightly increased freezing behavior (Fig. 5b-c) and significantly reduced max speed 2 sec upon VLS events (Fig. 5d-e). Noticeably, control mice significantly reduced the time spent in the nest after VLS presentation across sessions, whereas mice with GAD2IPN◊LDTg circuit photoinhibition continued spending a significant amount of time inside the nest over VLS days (Fig. 5f-g), suggesting that inhibition of this circuit occludes adaptive learning of innate threat responses. Time spent in other zones, such as the wall, safe, and trigger as well as latency to nest, were not influenced by optogenetic inhibition of the GAD2IPN◊LDTg circuit (Supplementary Fig. 9).
A subpopulation of IPN neurons expressing Sst encodes defensive responses in a threatening environment.
Within the IPN, a GABAergic subpopulation expressing somatostatin (Sst)(36) demonstrates a highly selective dorso-rostral to ventro-caudal gradient (Supplementary Fig. 10a). Sst is a neuropeptide typically co-released with GABA and involved in anxiety-like behaviors (37). To explore whether IPN Sst+ responded to VLS, we injected Cre-dependent GCaMP in the IPN of mice expressing Cre recombinase under the control of the Sst promoter (Fig. 6a and Supplementary Fig. 10b). We found that aversive stimuli such as VLS presentations (Fig. 6b-e), a tail lift (Supplementary Fig. 10c) or a foot shock (Supplementary Fig. 10d), triggered an increase in IPN Sst+ neuronal activity. Remarkably, VLS-induced IPN Sst+ activation occurred after reaching max speed upon VLS (Fig. 6b) and did not reduce from day 1 to day 3 (Fig. 6c-e). Interestingly, we also detected IPN Sst+ activation time-locked to nest entry, although these signals did not adjust across repeated VLS sessions (Fig. 6f-h). Further analysis demonstrated on day 1 of the looming test, the engagement of IPN Sst+ neurons with nest entry positively correlated with speed levels 10 sec upon VLS (Supplementary Fig. 10e) and time spent inside the nest (Supplementary Fig. 10f), while it negatively correlated with nest latency (Supplementary Fig. 10g) or time spent near the walls (Supplementary Fig. 10h). Overall, IPN Sst+ activation with nest entry consistently predicted VLS-induced defensive behaviors, suggesting these neurons may encode safety and avoidance aspects of threat processing.
Genetic ablation of IPN Sst + neurons reduces anxiety-like behaviors.
We next addressed the role of IPN Sst+ neurons in threat processing and defensive learning by genetically ablating this neuronal population using a Cre-dependent caspase 3 approach (Fig. 6i). Removing IPN Sst+ neuronal function did not affect VLS-induced freezing (Supplementary Fig. 11a-b) or changes in speed (Supplementary Fig. 11c-d) across the 3-day looming sessions. Nevertheless, we observed that IPN Sst+ ablated animals significantly reduced the time spent inside the nest as compared to mCherry control mice (Fig. 6j-k). In contrast, animals with IPN Sst ablation spent more time nearby the safety (Supplementary Fig. 11e-f) and wall zones (Supplementary Fig. 11g-h), without affecting nest latency (Supplementary Fig. 11i). The reduction in nest time observed by animals with ablated IPN Sst + neurons support the notation that these neurons play a role in processing avoidance-related aspects of threat-evoked defensive behavior, but not motor function. Along these lines, we also detected that IPN Sst+ ablation increased the time spent in the open arms of the elevated plus maze (Supplementary Fig. 11j) but not the number of closed arm entries (Supplementary Fig. 11k). Similarly, IPN Sst ablated animals showed increased time of center exploration in an open field test (Supplementary Fig. 11l) without altering locomotor activity in this assay (Supplementary Fig. 11m).