The Wood Warbler is known as a trans-Saharan migrant and at the same time as a nomadic species, since males do not return to the same place every year and often change territory during the breeding season, thus displaying low nest-site fidelity. However, the benefits of frequent change between territories are still widely discussed (Herremans 1993; Wesołowski et al. 2009; Szymkowiak and Kuczynski 2015). In field experiments we have shown that one of the reasons behind the tendency of males to leave the plot lies in their extra-pair mating behaviour rather than in the attempt to find a better nest-site.
In this study, males were divided into two groups, depending on their behaviour on a site:
1) Residents - males who settle on site for a long period of time;
2) Floaters - males who settle on site but leave after several days of singing.
Our initial hypothesis was that the emergence and departure of floaters is a result of their behaviour, in which males evaluate the environment during settlement and then leave the site if they find it unsuitable. To determine which males arrived and which left, we banded all residents in 2021 and 2022, as well as most residents and floaters in 2023.
As argued, wood warblers avoid high intraspecific competition by escaping settlement near highly competitive conspecifics. Previous experiments demonstrated the preference of males to settle on plots with ‘poor quality’ (PQ) male song broadcast and, on the contrary, to often leave plots with ‘good quality’ (GQ) male song broadcast (Szymkowiak et al. 2016). However, in our study, neither PQ nor GQ treatment plots were preferred by males for settlement, since resident abundance did not differ between plots (Fig. 1b).
The lack of evidence that song broadcasts increase the number of residents is not in stark contrast to the findings of other studies. Clustering of wood warbler territories is more evident at large scales, whereas at local scales it may be masked by bird territoriality (Broughton et al. 2020). Furthermore, settlement is a hierarchical decision-making process based on personal (nonsocial) information and social information obtained from observation of other conspecifics and their interaction with the environment (Danchin et al. 2004). The Wood Warbler does not prioritise social over nonsocial information during settlement and does not settle on sites of low quality regardless of the conspecific song broadcasted there (Luepold et al. 2023). In addition, some factors, such as the predator presence or rodent abundance, may also negatively influence bird settlement decisions (Szymkowiak and Kuczynski 2015; Szymkowiak and Thomson 2019). As a result, plots with higher numbers of residents could be of better quality and, therefore viewed by wood warblers as favourable for settlement due to some characteristics that researchers may underestimate. In our study, the resident abundance per plot was positively correlated with the number of residents in the previous and subsequent years, which allows us to assume that wood warblers preferred to settle on the same plots every year regardless of song broadcast.
On the other hand, the absence of difference in resident abundance between plots might derive from the relatively low population densities in the north, where our study was conducted (Lapshin 2009). However, the mean number of males ranged from 8.0 to 4.4 male per plot in 2021 and 2023 respectively and did not differ much from what was shown in other song playback experiments (Szymkowiak et al. 2016; Grendelmeier et al. 2016; Luepold et al. 2023). In addition, in the north wood warblers are less aggressive in establishing territorial boundaries (Matantseva et al. 2015), thus there may be no benefit for males to preferentially settle near PQ males and the resident abundance may not differ significantly between plots.
Given the limited favourable time for breeding in the north, we assumed that an adequate strategy for males is to settle and begin attracting females as soon as possible. Despite our expectations, between 52 and 66.7% of males left the plot within the first seven days after arrival. Based on the suggestion of previous study (Szymkowiak et al. 2016), a hypothesis was put forward that after leaving floaters could settle on song plots with PQ conspecifics. Consequently, the focus shifted towards finding these males again after their relocation. In 2023, 42 out of 79 males left the plot after we banded them, but only two (4.5%) wood warblers resettled on other plots at a distance of 1.5-2 km. Those males relocated from control plots to other control plots and bred there. During three years of research, there were no encounters of any other ringed floaters, despite the fact that we regularly visited all plots and adjacent territories (5.4 km2) throughout the entire breeding season. None of the ringed individuals were caught at the Ladoga Ornithological Station either. It is important to point out that the study areas were limited to Ladoga Lake and swamps, so males likely dispersed mainly along the coast and rivers, where the Ladoga ornithological station and our study area were located. The reason for why these males did not try to settle on PQ treatment plots seemed unclear.
More than half of the male population remains unmated and continues to arrive and leave within the season. According to other studies (Herremans 1993; Szymkowiak et al. 2016; Grendelmeier et al. 2016; Luepold et al. 2023), this phenomenon is observed even in the central part of the Wood Warbler's range. However, little is known about where these males came from. Another study has confirmed that most of the extra-pair offspring found in the nests were not related to neighbouring males and only in one case the extra-pair father was identified (Goretskaia et al. 2024). There was another suggestion that some males sing in secondary territories far up to 1.4 km from the primary territory and may participate in copulations with females from other pairs, and then return to their first mate in the nest (Goretskaia et al. 2024). However, our bird banding data showed that the newly arrived floaters were not polyterritorial males from a neighbouring territory, since most of them had not been ringed yet or found again after banding. In this study, a few males had a second territory and they always returned to the first site, where their females incubated the eggs. Other studies (Herremans 1993; Norman 1994) documenting the within-season movements of unmated males have also found only a few wood warblers, who moved to a new territory up to 14–32 km away from the previous location.
We tried to determine whether the number of floaters depends on the number of residents in the current or previous years, but did not find any significant correlation, except for treatment in 2021, when the floater abundance was more substantial on the GQ treatment plots. The experiment was conducted again in 2023 in another area but the results were not repeated. That brought up the assumption that using the same song recordings across plots may have resulted in pseudoreplication in 2021, therefore creating biased results, whereas in 2023 song recordings from different males were used. However, the phenomenon of attracting floaters to GQ treatment plots was in agreement with the results obtained in the central part of the Wood Warbler range (Szymkowiak et al. 2016). On the other hand, in 2023, only 76.5% of the plots in the DSF were occupied by wood warblers, despite the song playback, meaning that if there were no residents, then there were either few floaters or none at all. Hence the plots we selected might not be equally attractive to wood warbler as we mentioned above.
There is some experimental evidence suggesting that the presence of the common chiffchaff can also attract wood warblers and influence them to settle nearby (Szymkowiak et al. 2017). However, our results demonstrated that the common chiffchaff abundance was positively correlated with the number of residents and not with the number of floaters or the total number of males per plot. There is hardly any benefit for wood warblers to gather post-breeding social cues in the current year to make settlement decisions for the following year, since only 0.8% of ringed adult males returned next year and the return rate of ringed nestling is even lower (Sokolov et al. 1996; Lapshin 2009). It is conceivable that wood warblers may collect post-breeding cues during juvenile and autumn migration, far away from the current breeding site, but we do not have enough data to confirm this.
Clustering of male territories is another common feature of the Wood Warbler even though not all males in a cluster succeed in attracting females (Herremans 1993; Norman 1994; Grendelmeier et al. 2016; Luepold 2023). However, the ‘hidden lek’ hypothesis postulates that aggregations of territorial birds may as well increase their chances for extra-pair copulation (Wagner 1998; Fletcher and Miller 2006). Since each female is ready to be fertilised only during short periods of time, males should gather around the nest during egg-laying.
Despite the new clutch appearing asynchronously throughout the breeding season – from 22 May to 13 July (53 days), the fluctuations in male abundance were not caused only by the arrival of new birds from wintering grounds or the decreasing number of birds as the season progressed. Within each plot, nonlinear dynamics are observed: the number of males increases during the egg-laying in a local nest, and then decreases after the incubation start (Fig. 2), which can occur several times on the plot depending on the number of nests. However, the degree of the fluctuations decreases towards the end of the season. Both of these phenomena describe the local population dynamics within a season better than all factors independently (Table 3, model 5). Moreover, a greater increase in male abundance on a plot was predicted by the nest presence at the egg-laying stage (Table 2, model 5). During our field work, we could detect the female arrival by the appearance of new males in the aggregation, sometimes even before registering her presence ourselves. Furthermore, we observed that most unpaired males in aggregations leave the area immediately after the end of the copulation period.
So far, our results support the previous findings (Luepold 2023) that plot quality is a better predictor of resident abundance than song playback. Consequently, according to our hypothesis, the presence of paired males should also attract floaters to the plots. In this study, most of the residents have paired with female eventually. However, no correlation was found between residence and floater abundance in the current year. This was not surprising since not all floaters implement an opportunistic strategy. The difference between ‘residents’ and ‘floaters’ is not quite the same as between ‘conservative’ and ‘opportunistic’ males, as some males may be classified as floaters if they found the plot unsuitable, died or left the site too early due to nest depredation.
According to the ‘hidden lek’ hypothesis, some floaters do not reside on a permanent site but move through the territories of other males in order to copulate with as many females as possible. This leads to the question of why floaters may be attracted to GQ treatment plots with a high rate song broadcast. As it has been previously shown (Szymkowiak et al. 2016), males with a higher song rate are more likely to pair with a female. It is also known that a certain phrase of a song and its song rate are significantly associated with testosterone levels in wood warblers, suggesting that some males may sing more often but invest less time in guarding the female (Belokon et al. 2020). Thus, floaters are more likely to copulate with females who paired with a higher song rate singing males. However, this assumption requires further research.
The reasons of nomadism
The Wood Warbler is described as a nomadic species due to large interannual fluctuations in the bird’s local abundance and low nest-site fidelity (Norman 1994; Wesołowski et al. 2009; Teitelbaum and Mueller 2019). For some reasons most adult wood warblers do not return to the previous breeding site after wintering. Annual return rates of ringed adult birds varies greatly depending on the region and reaches a maximum of 28% in Western Europe (Norman 1994), while in Central and Eastern Europe only a few birds were found or recaptured again (Herremans 1993; Sokolov et al. 1996; Lapshin 2009). We strongly doubt that a high mortality rate could be the cause, since the species did not increase reproduction success to compensate for the loss of individuals (Wesołowski and Maziarz 2009; Maag et al. 2022), and it does not show any tendency towards extinction except in western territories (Vickery et al. 2014). Apart from that, the wood warblers exhibit plasticity in feeding behaviour (Herremans 1993; Maziarz and Wesołowski 2010; Mallord et al. 2017), so the distribution of food resources is not the cause of nomadic patterns either.
Local numbers of birds may fluctuate because wood warblers avoid settling in areas with a large number of rodents and predators that depredate their ground nests (Szymkowiak and Kuczynski 2015; Szymkowiak and Thomson 2019). The distribution of rodents may vary annually due to many factors, so wood warblers may adjust to these fluctuations, and thus display low site fidelity. Nevertheless, there is no large-scale increase in reproductive success (Wesołowski and Maziarz 2009; Lapshin 2020; Maag et al. 2022), so it is not obvious what benefit they receive compared to other ground-nesting species with high site fidelity. The number of wood warblers is negatively correlated with the abundance of bank vole Clethrionomys glareolus and yellow-necked mouse Apodemus flavicollis (Wesołowski et al. 2009; Gerber 2011), but there is no evidence that birds deliberately avoid rodents or may perceive their abundance using acoustic signals (Stelbrink et al. 2019). Whether they can use visual (Gerber 2011) or olfactory cues to estimate the local number of rodents is unknown.
In accordance with our data, we put forward the hypothesis that males have two reproductive strategies: conservative and opportunistic. Conservative males typically display nest-site fidelity and bond with females, while opportunistic males show low nest-site fidelity, regularly changing their territory shortly after copulation with a paired female. The opportunistic strategy aims to leave as many extra-pair offspring in local nests as possible and, as a result, it drives nomadic behaviour of males as well. This is in agreement with other studies that have shown that within-season movements of wood warblers are related to mate searching (Luepold et al. 2024), and that most fathers of extra-pair young are hard to identify because they have left the area (Goretskaia et al. 2024).
In 2021–2023, the population remained unstable during the breeding season, as between 52 and 66.7% of all detected males left the territory. Based on our assumption, at least part of the floaters implement opportunistic strategies, therefore, they do not pair with females during the breeding season and do not help rear offspring. Then, if the male does not have a permanent nest-site to attract a female, he does not need to return to the place where the researcher banded him. This behaviour partly explains why we observed so few ringed males returning in the following years.
For the Wood Warbler, which is considered to be a social monogamous species, the number of males adopting opportunistic strategies may depend on the following territorial conditions. Firstly, on territories where males greatly outnumber females. Secondly, on territories where birds arrive and start breeding in asynchrony. In central areas of the range the breeding period is relatively long and periods for the mate search is also longer than on the northern periphery of the range, where more males are forced to be involved in extra-pair mating rather than attracting a mate. So, it appears that in the most optimal habitats the return rate increases to 28% (Norman 1994), while in the north the return rate of males remains critically low (0.8% or less).
Although the mortality rate of females is usually higher than that of males in birds (Xirocostas et al. 2020; Payevsky 2021), the reasons for the greater predominance of wood warbler males over females compared to other passerine species are not entirely clarified. An additional explanation for the higher female mortality may be winter habitat segregation among the sexes in the Wood Warbler (Hobson et al. 2014). On the other hand, the constant movements of males should also distort the image of the actual sex ratio in the local population.
At the end of our study we were left with some interesting questions for further investigation: do males switch between strategies during the season or between years? Is there a genetic difference between males with different strategies? Our result also showed that there was no difference in the length of the wing and tail between residents and floaters, as well as in similar results obtained in another study, when paired and unpaired males were compared (Herremans 1993). It would be curious to determine if first-year males are more likely to try to attract a female, while older males likely switch to an opportunistic strategy. This could be the factor that reduces the return rate of successfully nesting birds in following years as well. The main difficulty we face is that we cannot determine yet, where the floaters fly away to within the breeding season and next year. Apart from that, another problem lies in determining the age of wood warblers, which occurs due to the complete moult during the winter season. The number of extra-pair offspring also appears to vary greatly between studies (Grendelmeier et al. 2016; Goretskaia et al. 2024), which may be due to the use of different markers to determine kinship estimation in a given species. Thus, these outcomes point to the need for further investigation of the extra-pair involvement of floaters in the reproduction of the young and their contribution to the genetic diversity of the population.