Unit participation and demographics
Forty-seven of the 50 UKCRC registered CTUs were represented at the network meeting on 28th April 2018. Of those present, 34 representatives from 31 Units (62%) participated. Following the meeting, Units without a completed survey were contacted, of which thirteen responded (n = 13/19). Supplementary Table 1 provides further detail. The overall participation rate of registered Units was 88% (n = 44/50). One representative from a newly registered Unit reported lack of experience as a reason for non-participation, reasons were not provided from the remaining five Units.
All responders had a statistical background with the majority of responders holding a senior or lead at their Unit (senior statistician: n = 15/44, 34%; statistical lead: n = 13/44, 30%). Supplementary Table 2 provides further detail.
Units listed on the UKCRC Resource Finder [11] as conducting cluster or surgical trials had participation rates 94% (n = 16/17) and 92% (n = 33/36) respectively (Supplementary Table 3).. Units with a methodological research area in complex interventions participated with a rate of 90% (n = 35/39). The statistical roles of
Three-quarters of Units indicated experience in running trials with a complex intervention (n = 32/44, 73%) and two-thirds in running trials with a surgical intervention (n = 29/44, 66%), with twenty-five (57%) indicating experience in both. Seven Units stated that their Unit did not have experience in running trials with either type of intervention (n = 7/44, 16%). One did not respond to this question (Question 1, Supplementary Table 4)..
Managing effects through design
Clustering
Twenty-five Units had undertaken multicentre trials that did not stratify by centre (n = 25/44, 57%, Question 2, Table 1 and Table 2).. Common reasons for not stratifying by centre were many centres with few participants (n = 19/25, 76%) and expected homogeneity of treatment effect (n = 11/25, 44%). Additional reasons for not stratifying by centre included allocation concealment in an open trial; logistical reasons; and grouping centres by region. One responder clearly indicated that this decision was influenced by the nature of the intervention stating:
"…drug trials less effect due to centre compared to say complex or surgical interventions.” [ID23]
One responder did stratify all their trials by centre alluded to concerns regarding potential for unequal distribution of costs across centres:
"This subject gets a lot of academic debate in some academic circles. But: our randomisation defaults to stratifying by centre; need to balance resources—don’t want to give one too many overheads; balancing avoids confounding; other opinions, such as Torgerson, exist.” [ID8]
Question 3 asked responders to consider five scenarios (Box 1, Table 1 and Supplementary Table 5),, in particular their approach to stratifying the randomisation in trials of each type ran by their Unit. Responses to Scenario A, of which 39 Units had experience, indicated that most Units when running a trial with a large sample size, with multiple treatment providers per centre each recruiting a minimum of 10 participants, would stratify by centre alone (n = 31/39, 87%).
Three would stratify by treatment provider alone (n = 3/39, 8%). Seventy percent had experience of running trials like Scenario B, which was the same as Scenario A, only with a small sample size (n = 31/44, 70%). As with Scenario A, most Units ran such trials by stratifying by centre alone (n = 24/31, 77%) and few by treatment provider alone (n = 2/31, 6%).
Responders had less experience running Scenario C trials, trials recruiting in several centres where treatment providers treated patients across centres (n = 16/44, 36%). Again, most common was stratification by centre only (n = 14/16, 88%), with a greater number of Units indicating that they had stratified such trials by treatment provider only (n = 3/16, 19%).
Units with experience running trials in Scenario D, trials recruiting from multiple centres, each with multiple treatment providers, that investigated a surgical intervention (n = 25/44, 57%), also primarily stratified by centre only (n = 21/25, 84%). One-fifth indicated stratifying by both centre and treatment provider in such trials (n = 5/25, 20%).
Whilst Units had less experience running trials like Scenario E, which was similar to Scenario D but investigating substantially different interventions, stratification approaches were similar to Scenario D (Centre only: 13/16, 81%; both centre and treatment provider: 2/16, 13%).
Twelve responders provided free text explaining their approaches for stratification in each of the scenarios (Question 3, Supplementary Table 5).. Two-thirds (n = 8/12, 67%) commented on the feasibility of stratifying by treatment provider. Reasons were: concerns that there would be too few per strata [ID8, ID15, ID39]; treatment provider not known in advance [ID8, ID32]; delivered by a subset of treatment deliverers [ID1, ID39]; data not collected on treatment provider [ID13]; treatment differences assumed to be differences in facilities and protocols [ID17]; usually comparing the intervention policy and not the different aspects of the intervention [ID32]; treatment provider can change during the trial [ID30].
Other responses provided examples of stratification levels e.g. centre as hospital and treatment provider as operating surgeon [ID10]; two that this was trial specific [ID14, ID29]. One raised concerns with stratifying by centre:
"Recent conversions between senior statisticians advocate not stratifying by centre in any situation. They cited concerns regarding prediction of allocation.” [ID18]
When comparing stratification approaches across scenarios within Units (Question 3, Table 1),, nineteen Units used the same approach across all scenarios they had experience in and twenty changed their approach depending on the trial scenario (same: n = 19/44, 43%; different: n = 20/44, 46%). Five had no experience in any of the suggested scenarios or did not respond to the question.
Learning
The majority of responders (n = 39/44, 89%) indicated they had accounted for learning by defining a minimum level of expertise for treatment providers (Question 4, Table 1).. Common definitions were set in terms of delivering the trial intervention (n = 31/44, 70%); treating the condition within the patient population (n = 24/44, 55%); and setting a minimum professional level for treatment providers (n = 22/44, 50%). Three delegated this responsibility to the clinical investigators on the study. Examples of alternative approaches to specifying minimum levels of expertise included: use of a surgical manual with senior surgeons signing off treatment deliverers [ID16] and treatment deliverers being required to pass both surgical and radiotherapy quality assurance [ID18].
Thirty percent of Units had used an expertise based trial design, in which participating treatment providers provide only the intervention in which they have expertise (n = 13/44, 30%, Question 5, Table 1)..
Managing effects through analysis
Clustering
In trials stratified by centre, 55% of Units had subsequently adjusted by this stratification factor in the analysis (n = 24/44, 55%, Question 6, Table 3 and Table 4).. This had been done either by pre-specified grouping rules at the design stage (n = 19/24, 83%); by an ad-hoc approach (n = 14/24, 58%); or by other approaches: grouped centres where numbers are small [ID7, ID15]; site as a fixed effect [ID8]; or:
"Depends. Either include as a stratifying factor (small number of centres, large patient numbers) or by including centre or treatment provider as a cluster.”” [ID32]
Regardless of stratification approach used, very few Units had never adjusted for centre in the statistical model when comparing treatment (n = 3/44, 7%, Question 7, Table 3 and Supplementary Box 3).. Responders from Units that did (39/44, 89%), did so using fixed effects (n = 11); random effects (n = 12); or, depending on the circumstance, used either (n = 14). Two did not respond. Reasons in favour for fixed effects were ease of interpretation and less assumptions associated with it, [ID27]; and random effects as:
“Usually an underlying assumption that centre may be a surrogate for socioeconomic factors that may affect outcome and/or treatment effect and so often not happy to assume that there is an equal fixed treatment effect across all sites.” [ID16]
In trials stratified by treatment provider, 37% also subsequently adjusted the analysis (n = 16/44, 37%, Question 6, Table 3 and Table 4).. Three-quarters did so in accordance with pre-specified grouping rules (n = 12/16, 75%) or using a more ad hoc approach (n = 7/16, 44%).
Regardless of stratification approach used, 59% adjust for treatment provider in the statistical model when comparing treatment (n = 26/44, 59%, Question 8, Table 3 and Supplementary Box 4).. The majority of responders used a random effect (n = 18/26, 69%), with one providing reason:
“If treatment provider was included as stratification factor it will be because we are concerned that the provider will have an impact on outcome but also because we would expect different population for different treatment providers.” [ID16]
When responders were asked to revisit the scenarios in Box 1, this timeto consider investigating treatment by centre or treatment provider (Question 9, Table 3),, exploring treatment by centre was universally most common across all scenarios. Exploring treatment by provider was rare. Twelve responders provided free text to explain their approaches for adjustment (Question 9, Supplementary Table 6).. General themes for additional information provided were: that the decision is trial dependent [ID6, ID14]; concerns around sample size [ID6, ID7, ID39]; and, when explored, that this was informal. [ID5, ID8, ID14, ID32, ID38]
When comparing treatment interaction approaches across scenarios within Units (Question 9, Table 3),, 24 Units used the same approach across all scenarios and twelve utilised a scenario specific approach (same: n = 24/44, 56%; different: n = 12/44, 27%). Eight had no experience in any of the suggested scenarios or did not respond to the question.
Seventy-three percent of Units explore heterogeneity by centre when a positive treatment effect is found (n = 32/44, 73%, Question 10a, Table 3),, whereas fewer explored heterogeneity by treatment provider (n = 12/44, 27%, Question 10b, Table 3).. Of those that do explore heterogeneity for either effect, the majority did so by graphical display (centre: n = 31/32; treatment provider: n = 11/12). Many also explored by analytical methods, for example significance testing (centre: n = 22/32; treatment provider: n = 9/12). Supplementary Tables 6 and 7 provides further detail.
Learning
Fifty-nine percent of Units included the treatment provider in the statistical model when comparing treatment (n = 26/44, 59%), two of which had treated this as a time-varying covariate (Question 8, Table 3),, with one specifying:
“Fairly crude by letting the number of procedures in the trial increase the relevant surgeon’s experience (ignoring procedures done outside of the trial of course!)" [ID38]
Those that had not used a time varying effect had experience of exploring learning through a sensitivity analysis [ID35] or secondary analyses [ID8, ID39] to check for learning effect exploring learning effects with neither being significant. The latter adding that:
“Had we found evidence of learning, we would have had awkward additional data summaries and presentations”
Two responders had not considered such analyses [ID7, ID23] and one provided time restrictions as a reason for not doing so [ID30].