The COVID-19 pandemic impacted families in many ways, including the loss of close loved ones and increased layoffs and demotions that resulted in trauma, financial instability, and economic stress (Katikireddi et al., 2021). Importantly, there were also exaggerated levels of hostility and discrimination towards persons from ethnic/racial minoritized heritage in the U.S., resulting in psychological and behavioral consequences (FBI, 2020). Given prior evidence that stress can undermine prosocial behaviors (i.e., actions of kindness and generosity that benefit others; Davis & Carlo, 2019), it is reasonable to assume that pandemic-related economic stress could be negatively associated with prosocial behaviors toward cultural heritage out-group members.
Prosocial behaviors are integral to harmonious and cooperative intergroup relationships and represent healthy social development (see Carlo, 2014; Davis et al., 2021). There are six different types of prosocial behaviors that are relatively representative of youth helping behaviors (Carlo, 2014): emotional, dire, compliant, anonymous, public, and altruistic. Emotional prosocial behaviors are helping others who are upset, such as comforting. Dire prosocial behaviors refer to helping in high intensity situations. Compliant prosocial behaviors are responding in a helpful way to requests for help. Public prosocial behaviors are helping behaviors done with an audience. Anonymous prosocial behaviors are helping when others are unaware, such as donating time or resources. Finally, altruistic prosocial behaviors include helping with little to no expected benefit to the self (Carlo, 2014).
Of particular interest to understanding prosocial behaviors directed towards cultural heritage out-group members are public and altruistic prosocial behaviors. Because public helping is conducted with the knowledge that others are watching, it is thought to be relatively more self-serving and instrumental (Carlo et al., 2003). Indeed, there is evidence that public prosocial behaviors are positively linked to approval-oriented prosocial moral reasoning—an egoistically-motivated form of reasoning (see Carlo, 2014). In contrast, altruistic prosocial behaviors are often costly to one’s self and considered relatively selfless because there is little or no expected self-reward. Indeed, altruistic helping has been linked to principled and empathic prosocial moral reasoning and to altruistic resource allocations in behavioral task measures (e.g., Benenson et al., 2007). Other research has yielded evidence that altruistic, public, emotional, dire, compliant, and anonymous forms of helping are related but distinct factors (see Carlo, 2014).
Despite the multitude of research on distinct forms of prosocial behaviors, what has yet to be examined thoroughly in the literature is how the person needing help (target) might influence whether or not people engage in prosocial behaviors across situations. It may be that experiencing pandemic-related economic stress predicts helping racial out-group members differently than racial in-group members, in part because of the desire to draw close to similar others during times of uncertainty and stress (Choi & Hogg, 2020). The study of prosocial behaviors during a pandemic, therefore, can inform our understanding of intergroup behaviors under adverse circumstances. The primary purpose of the present study was to examine the links between pandemic-related economic stress and out-group prosocial behaviors and to determine whether those links could be accounted for by individual differences in ethnic identity and filial piety.
Culturally-Grounded Theoretical Frameworks
Based on prior conceptual and empirical work, two culturally integrative models of prosocial development and social inequities were adopted for the present study (Carlo & de Guzman, 2009; Davis et al., 2021). These models identify different forms of stress as influences on youth prosocial behaviors. Moreover, the models suggest that culture-group characteristics, such as ethnic identity and traditional cultural values, can intervene in the links between economic stress and multiple forms of prosocial behaviors (Davis et al., 2021). Although these models were developed with Latino/ a youth, they can apply across ethnic and cultural groups (Davis et al., 2021). Additionally, stress and coping theorists suggest that pervasive stress can compromise coping resources over time and contribute to maladjustment (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Alternatively, the altruism-born-of-suffering hypothesis (Staub & Vollhardt, 2008), for example, asserts that when persons experience stress, they may become increasingly sensitive to the needs of others and may be more inclined to engage in helpful behaviors because of their motivation for reducing others’ suffering. In other words, it might be that stress fosters an understanding of the experiences of others, which could contribute to selfless forms of helping.
Links Between Economic Stress and Prosocial Behaviors
Economic stress is a form of stress that impacts the family system as well as intrapersonal cognitive processes, such as self-regulation and coping strategies, and can lead to deleterious consequences for young adults (Conger et al., 1992; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Understanding economic stress during young adulthood might be particularly important because of the nature of this developmental period. Young adults are navigating increasingly complicated responsibilities and relationships across multiple domains (e.g., career, social relationships, financial independence; Arnett, 2020). Therefore, understanding the role of pandemic-related stress on youth positive social development during this developmental period is important.
While research on economic stress and prosocial behaviors is limited, few relevant studies exist. There is some evidence that economic stress is negatively associated with prosocial behaviors globally as well as dire, emotional, and compliant prosocial behaviors (Carlo et al., 2011; Davis & Carlo, 2019). However, one recent study found that economic stress was positively associated with prosocial behaviors toward friends for young adults who were also high in community self-efficacy, but the link was negative for young adults who were low in community self-efficacy (Davis et al., 2022). Another study found that economic stress was positively related to selflessly-motivated prosocial behaviors but negatively related to selfishly-motivated prosocial behaviors (Davis et al., 2017). The present study extends this literature by examining economic stress experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic while also examining prosocial behaviors directed specifically toward cultural heritage out-group members. In addition, we investigated the possible intervening roles of two culture-group-related traits, ethnic identity and filial piety, in the relations between economic stress and multiple forms of out-group prosocial behaviors.
Links Between Economic Stress and Ethnic Identity
Social identity theory suggests that group membership drives one’s self concept because of the meaning attached to social groups (Tajfel & Turner, 2004). One form of social identity that is quite salient to U.S. ethnic/racial minoritized persons is ethnic identity. Ethnic identity refers to integrating one’s ethnic heritage into one's self-concept through the processes of exploration, commitment, and attachment (Phinney & Ong, 2007). Stressful experiences might promote ethnic identity because of the desire to draw close to one’s social group during contentious times as a coping strategy (see Yip et al., 2008). Therefore, it may be that ethnic identity is salient for marginalized youth, and frequent experiences of stress could strengthen one’s ethnic identity.
Although there is evidence that ethnic identity can be a moderator in stress research (e.g., Umaña-Taylor et al., 2011), there is also substantive evidence that ethnic identity is directly related to stress. For example, there is evidence that stress experiences, including discrimination, promote ethnic identity and cultural values among Latino/a youth (Brittian et al., 2013; Meca et al., 2020). How pandemic-related economic stress might predict ethnic identity across diverse ethnic groups, including White, European American youth, is not clear.
Interrelations Between Ethnic Identity and Filial Piety
An important value found in many cultures, particularly emphasized in collectivistic societies, is filial piety (Fuligni et al., 1999). Filial piety is defined as close ties with one’s family that manifest in care-giving behaviors, loyalty to family, and respect and honor associated with the family unit (Yeh & Bedford, 2003). While filial piety has traditionally been studied in Asian and Latino/a populations, there is also research examining variation in this construct across diverse populations in the United States, including White, European heritage populations (Fuligni et al., 1999). Ethnic identity reflects internalized feelings about one’s own ethnic group, so young adults who feel positive about and connected to their ethnic group might also strongly treat family relationships as paramount (Kiang & Fuligni, 2009).
There is substantive supportive longitudinal and cross-sectional evidence that ethnic identity is positively associated with cultural values, including familism (e.g., Crockett et al., 2022; Knight et al., 2016; Streit et al., 2020). For example, a large study of U.S. Latino/a, Asian American, and European American adolescents found that ethnic identity was strongly associated with a higher endorsement of family obligation and assistance (Kiang & Fuligni, 2009). Consistent with these findings, we expect that ethnic identity will positively predict filial piety, as filial piety is a central element of familism, and both reflect communal orientations (Schwartz et al., 2010).
Links Between Filial Piety and Prosocial Behaviors
Cultural socialization and prosocial development theories highlight the important role of other-oriented values and values that prioritize the family as predictors of higher levels of prosocial behaviors (Carlo & Conejo, 2019). Scholars have theorized that cultural values that center the family as a top priority, including filial piety, might promote perspective-taking and empathic responding in youth because these values require youth to think about broader social goals and the needs of family members over the self (Calderón-Tena et al., 2011). Additionally, the family context is a primary source of socialization for prosocial behaviors, as family members provide initial opportunities to practice prosocial behaviors while also creating an environment that either fosters internalization of values or does not, ultimately shaping young adults’ prosocial tendencies (Eisenberg, 2004; Hastings et al., 2007).
It might be that filial piety, which is tied to valuing the needs of others, is most likely positively related to care-based, other-oriented forms of prosocial behaviors such as emotional, dire, and compliant prosocial behaviors. Furthermore, filial piety could facilitate public prosocial behaviors that entail gaining the approval of family members, which would be deemed socially appropriate. However, prosocial behaviors that require pre-planning (anonymous) or a cost to the self (altruistic) are moral principled- or empathic/sympathy-based; thus, filial piety might be either non-significantly or negatively related to those specific forms of prosocial behaviors.
There is a substantive body of research differentially linking familism values to specific forms of prosocial behaviors (e.g., Davis et al., 2021; Knight et al., 2018). For example, Knight et al. (2016) showed that familism was longitudinally, positively linked to compliant, dire, emotional, anonymous, and public, but negatively linked to altruistic, prosocial behaviors (see also Armenta et al., 2010). There is also research specifically linking filial piety (rather than familism) and prosocial behaviors, although this research is more limited in scope (Zhu et al., 2023). A study of Chinese and Indonesian young adults and adults found that filial piety was positively associated with prosocial behaviors (Zhen & Guo, 2021). Additionally, among Chinese adolescents, filial piety predicted volunteering through empathy (Yuan et al., 2023). While there is some evidence that filial piety might predict prosocial behaviors, more research is needed to understand how filial piety might predict specific forms of prosocial behaviors, especially across multiple ethnic groups in the United States.
Study Goals and Hypotheses
The current study aimed to assess the serial intervening roles of ethnic identity and filial piety in the links between economic stress during the COVID-19 pandemic and out-group prosocial behaviors. We hypothesized that pandemic-related economic stress would positively predict ethnic identity, which would positively predict filial piety. Filial piety, in turn, would positively predict out-group emotional, dire, anonymous, public, and compliant prosocial behaviors but might negatively predict altruistic prosocial behaviors. We also hypothesized that pandemic-related economic stress would directly negatively predict multiple forms of out-group prosocial behaviors but be positively linked to public prosocial behaviors because gaining the approval of family members is deemed acceptable.
We also examined whether the proposed model might differ as a function of ethnic majority/minority group status due to the potential differences in the centrality of ethnic identity and filial piety for ethnic majority and ethnic minority participants. Because ethnic identity is often more salient and protective for ethnic minority youth than the majority European American youth (Phinney et al., 1994), this variable might more strongly predict out-group prosocial behavior for ethnic minority young adults. Considering the role of ethnic identity and filial piety as explanatory variables in the links between stress and prosocial behaviors among ethnic majority youth has not been examined in the literature, these findings will contribute to a more nuanced understanding of how these cultural processes operate for diverse youth.