This study demonstrates the comparison of locomotor behaviors between two different fish models, Javanese medaka and zebrafish for 6 min using OFT. This assay is crucial in determining whether both fish species have similar baseline activity in an open field-based paradigm. Three different time frames were used to measure the net activity for both fish species. Early phase was analyzed as exploratory behavior while the remaining time, (mid and final phase) were used to evaluate the habituation responses. Results showed that Javanese medaka steadily increased their locomotor activity with lower anxiety responses in a novel-based paradigm. Meanwhile, zebrafish displayed a decreased in locomotor activity during early phase with higher anxiety responses.
Exploration plays an important role in animal’s natural behavior that is mainly used to investigate novel environments and to sustain their survivability (Baker et al., 2018). Interestingly, Javanese medaka took a longer duration before exploring the OFT tank. These findings suggest that Javanese medaka were characterized as passive individuals since they displayed shorter swimming distance and lower exploratory activities. In comparison to Javanese medaka, zebrafish have bolder behavioral phenotypes in open field environment since they rapidly swam throughout the novel arena, thus resulting in longer swimming distance. Boldness and shyness primarily denote the willingness among individual to take risk particularly in novel environment (Sih et al., 2004; Mustafa et al., 2019). The apparent disparity behavior between fish species may have arisen due to variation between domesticated (laboratory) zebrafish and wild Javanese medaka population. This findings corroborate with previous studies where Hutter et al., (2011) found that wild-derived zebrafish were more shy and distressed in captivity, whereas the domesticated zebrafish were regularly bold and eagerly approach human and other stimuli (Wright et al., 2006; Moretz et al., 2007). Besides, the captive environment which was highly stable with the absent of predators likely results in rapid domestication that leads to increase in boldness behavior in all captive vertebrates (Huntingford, 2004; Matsunaga and Watanabe, 2010; Agnvall et al., 2015; Rojas-Carvajal et al., 2018; Mustafa et al., 2019). Noteworthy, the difference in boldness and risk taking behavior of wild-type Javanese medaka versus domesticated zebrafish could also have been influenced by their genetic variation (Wright et al., 2006; Norton et al., 2011). However, further study should be conducted in the future to evaluate the differences of exploratory behavior for domesticated Javanese medaka versus wild caught Javanese medaka. A constant in the locomotor activity over time also implicated that the exploratory activity started to diminish due to exhibition of habituation response (Rankin et al., 2009).
Habituation is defined as a behavioral response decrement which results from repetitive stimulation without involving sensory adaptation/sensory fatigue or motor fatigue (Rankin et al., 2009; Thompson, 2018). Notably, Javanese medaka took slightly longer time to habituate in the OFT tank, while zebrafish habituated faster to the OFT environment. Different rate of habituation within populations are probably shaped by natural selection which influenced by animal’s adaptation towards its natural habitat (Bell and Peeke, 2012). A steady habituation in Javanese medaka might be due to their preferences for shallow periphery of the rivers where water current is slower and away from any point of disturbances (Yusof et al., 2013). In contrast, rapid habituation observed in zebrafish likely due to their natural habitat selection in the wild. They inhabit a wide range of habitats, including still, slow-flowing and fast-flowing water bodies (Suriyampola et al., 2016). Animals that experienced a more complex rearing environment during their development will habituate faster to novelty (Zimmermann et al., 2001). Moreover, this active learning process may help fish to distinguish false alarms to harmless events involving different mechanisms depending on stimulation, sensory pathway, and signal processing which eventually trigger various exploration responses in fish (Raderschall et al., 2011; Godwin et al., 2012; Gaspary et al., 2018). Variations in natural habitat preference and adaptation for different fish species reduce the risk of predation and increase their survivability. Natural habitat variations between two different fish species might influenced their habituations response in the OFT tank.
Thigmotaxis is a preference of animals towards the periphery of a novel arena and avoiding center area which ideally used as anxiety index (Schnörr et al., 2012). In the present study, Javanese medaka were found to be passive and have lower anxiety responses in comparison to zebrafish that were active and more aggressive, with higher anxiety responses. A significant edge preference started to be displayed by both fish species after 3 min, suggesting that the ideal time to analyze thigmotaxis behavior in OFT should be measured starting from fourth mins in OFT. The early phase of behavioral recording in a novel environment is less suitable for thigmotaxis assessment due to the influence of exploratory behavior (Gould et al., 2009). Like human and rodents, anxiety have been linked to specific behavior phenotypes where the organism avoid open spaces, searching for safety in enclosed space as a defensive response, lack of motivation to explore and also sometimes associated with cognitive dysfunction (Walz et al., 2016; Higaki et al., 2018). Since teleost also shared evolutionary conserved neural circuits in regard to anxiety-like behaviors (Belzung and Philippot, 2007; Stewart et al., 2012), both of fish models in this study clearly displayed thigmotaxic behavior. The preference to the periphery proposed that both fish species utilized vertical surfaces of the OFT wall as a spatial clue for navigation. This similarity of behavioral strategies for both fish species and rodents in novel environment suggest that OFT walls serve as a guiding and attractive force on locomotion which modulates their direction and speed in reference to walls during OFT navigation (Horev et al., 2007). These forms of attraction were suggested involving recognition and locational memory which may also underlie the Javanese medaka and zebrafish locomotion found in this study (Stewart et al., 2010). However, anxiety-related neuroendocrine responses should be taken into consideration in the future study by analyzing whole-body cortisol levels (Yeh et al., 2013). This will further confirm whether exposure to open field environment altered anxiety responses in both fish species, physiologically.
Furthermore, this study discovered that the formation of the home-base for both fish species were established at the edge of the OFT. The finding revealed that both fish species have one prominent home-base formation that was commonly established close to the OFT walls. Worth mentioning, Javanese medaka takes slightly longer duration to establish a home-base in comparison to zebrafish. A distinct formation of home-base started to form once they were habituated to the OFT tank. The home-base is denoting as a place in the area for which the experimental animal shows a preference across time, which typically exhibited in open field-based paradigm (Rosemberg et al., 2011; Stewart et al., 2012. Similar type of home-base formation also has been documented in rodent studies (Horev et al., 2007; Thompson et al., 2018).
With respect to other motor responses in OFT, both fish species displayed a balance left and right body orientations in order to navigate and explore OFT tank. No significant different in the percentage of frontal-vertical body posture towards the OFT walls were observed for both fish species. This frontal-vertical body posture suggested that the fish exhibit a pause and look behavior. This behavior is an important aspect of exploration that allows information gathering which required for decision making during their exploration in novel environment (Kalueff et al., 2014; Redish, 2016). It is worth to note that the formation of fish reflection on the OFT wall made up of transparent acrylic sheet during the behavioral recording was observed. This study was accidentally found that zebrafish responded aggressively to its own reflection on the wall of OFT tank in comparison to Javanese medaka. Nonetheless, this study was unable to determine whether the fish can view its own reflection on the acrylic sheet. Therefore, to determine whether the fish have self-recognition, this visually mediated behaviors can be further explored for future research by tracking eye movement using optokinetic reflex (OKR), oculomotor behavior and a built in customized paradigm specialized for self-recognition assessment should be as well implement (Maurer, et al., 2011; Dehmelt et al., 2018).
This study also assessed fish body movement (clockwise or counterclockwise) to determine which eye (left or right, respectively) was used to view the edge of the OFT wall. Both Javanese medaka and zebrafish significantly displayed lower than 50% of clockwise body movement throughout early, mid and final phase, suggesting that both fish species have strong preference to use their left eye. No significant differences were observed for anticlockwise orientation (right eye) in both fish species (additional file 3). In accordance with the present study, previous studies have reported that adult zebrafish used their left eyes to assess the novelty of objects or environment whereas right eyes were used for prey detection (Miklósi, et al., 1997; Sovrano, 2004; Watkins et al., 2004; Sovrano and Andrew, 2006).
This study showed that 6 min duration for behavioral recording in OFT was sufficient for both Javanese medaka and zebrafish to exhibit a visible thigmotaxis and home-bases behavior. A previous study showed that zebrafish exhibited a home-base formation after 30 min observation time (Stewart et al., 2011). However, longer OFT recording time may be applicable to enhance the characterization of locomotor that lead to multiple complex behaviors in both Javanese medaka and zebrafish that remain to be discovered. Although top view for video recording provides an accurate tracking and quantification for both Javanese medaka and zebrafish behavior, further studies may utilize multiple cameras to generate three-dimensional behavior tracking. Moreover, manipulation of neuropharmacological agents will helps to increase our knowledge about home-base and locomotor phenotypes in both fish species. A previous study has shown that rodents demonstrated alteration in home-base specific locations when drug were given in different doses (Dvorkin et al., 2010). Therefore, the sensitivity of both fish species towards neuropharmacological treatment on their behavior in novel OFT merits further investigation.