Social conformity—the modification of opinions and decisions to match those of others—is a fundamental aspect of human behavior and decision-making1. This is especially significant in today's online environment, where others’ opinions are ubiquitous in the form of recommendations, ratings, and reviews about products, services, etc.2. Conformity is driven by different underlying motivations, such as the desire to form precise perceptions of reality or to be “right”, and the desire to create and preserve relationships with others or to be “liked”3. These motivations often coexist, particularly when employing majority opinions, as the rule of the majority not only highlights the unpleasantness of standing alone but also underscores a rational foundation for this compromise: a collective group is statistically more likely to make an accurate decision than an individual4–6.
However, when the source of influence and the nature of the relationship between the individual and the social source are specified, the underlying motivation for influence can be driven by one of the motivations, depending on the context and the source of influence. In a study by Klucharev and colleagues7, pairing an expert celebrity with a product that matched their expertise produced better product recognition and a positive attitude toward the product in the later day’s assessment, compared to when the expert celebrity was linked to a product that did not match their reputation. Similarly, more recent research demonstrated that participants who listened to a health expert's narrative about the risks of sugar consumption significantly decreased their willingness to pay for sugar-containing foods compared to the control group8. Research by Engelmann and colleagues9 further supports this perspective by showing that risk-averse advice from a financial expert on a certainty equivalent task influenced the decision-making process in adults and adolescents. In a task requiring particular knowledge (price information of rental apartments), expert advice compared to non-expert advice significantly influenced the subject’s opinions and enhanced their judgment accuracy10. In the mentioned studies, perceived credibility and reliability of the expert sources were key drivers of conformity and positive attitude, especially when compared to scenarios where expertise was irrelevant or when expert sources were compared to control and novice groups.
Individuals rely on peer group judgments to achieve accurate knowledge11, however, they are frequently motivated to align with their peer group's norms and expectations to gain approval or avoid negative consequences such as social exclusion12–15. Empirical evidence demonstrates that during adolescence, individuals are more prone to engage in risky behaviors when accompanied by peers compared to when they are alone or with adults 16,17. This indicates that their motivation is not due to a lack of knowledge about appropriate actions, but rather a strong desire to be liked and accepted. Neuroimaging studies further corroborate this notion, revealing that conformity to peer norms shares neural activities associated with social exclusion 12, and experiencing social exclusion predicts subsequent conformity to peer norms 13.
While the studies mentioned earlier have examined the effect of peer and expert separately, a study with two social sources18 demonstrated that in an investment decision task, participants followed the advice of a financial expert significantly longer than the peer advice, despite identical and randomized suggestions. Similarly, another study19 with a probabilistic learning task and a within-subject design revealed that participants favored stimuli endorsed by experienced adults over those recommended by peers. A further study2 observed that when expert and peer recommendations conflicted in a profile picture selection task, participants were more influenced by expert endorsements, especially when these endorsements were positive. In these studies, social influence hinges on the reliability of the social source and quality of the advice rather than being driven by peer norms. Indeed, when the accuracy of an opinion is the main objective, people value and follow seemingly reliable advice and recommendations rather than following peer opinion 20,21. However, in a subjective evaluative judgment task where feedback on performance was provided by peers and experts rather than advice or recommendations for decision-making, adolescent participants conformed similarly to peer and expert groups22. This suggests that the context and task demands influence the effectiveness of peer and expert opinions. Specifically, expert influence may dominate in contexts requiring accuracy and informed decision-making and peer influence may prevail when there is an explicit identity component or a need to fit in.
A large body of neuroimaging studies has identified conflict monitoring as a crucial component within the neurocognitive network that underlies social conformity when there is a discrepancy between an individual’s opinion and peer group 4,23,24. One explanation for this conflict detection mechanism is that social conformity is based on reinforcement learning, suggesting that when an individual’s judgment contradicts the group consensus, a negative prediction error is triggered in the medial frontal cortex (MFC) and ventral striatum (VS). Conversely, when an individual’s judgment aligns with the group, reward signaling in the brain is amplified, reinforcing consistent opinions 4. In addition to the reinforcement learning framework of social conformity, Berns et al demonstrated in their study a significant positive correlation between specific neural activation (anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and anterior insula (AI)) and adolescents' propensity to conform to peer group norms in the context of evaluating music popularity12. The areas involved in this type of conformity to peers also exhibited similar neural activity during social exclusion, suggesting that such activation may reflect the perceived threat of social rejection and the consequent need for behavioral adjustment to maintain group harmony13. In a study with two experts as social sources, agreement with the experts on music preference was reflected in the activity of the brain regions associated with reward processing 25. In the study by Klucharev and colleagues, pairing an expert celebrity with a product that matched their expertise led to better product recognition and a positive attitude toward the product in the later assessment7. This expertise-congruent pairing was accompanied by increased activation in brain regions involved in reward processing and memory formation.
In studies comparing brain responses to two different social sources, where two differentially credible sources, a financial expert versus a peer source were compared. Participants who were exposed to the expert advice exhibited greater activation in the ACC and superior frontal gyrus regions when they chose to ignore the expert’s advice compared to those who disregarded the peer advice18. The activation of the ACC when ignoring the expert’s advice was considered an indicator of error detection and conflict monitoring, signaling the need to adjust future behavior based on the expert’s advice. The ACC activation was accompanied by superior frontal gyrus activation which is linked to certainty-related processing, meaning participants in the expert group may have required more certainty that disobeying the advice was the rational decision to make before choosing that action18. In the study comparing expert advice to novice advice, expert advice was used more by participants compared to the novices’ and judgment accuracy was higher in the expert group compared to the non-expert, and this preference correlated with activity in the ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), brain regions associated with reward processing10.
The main body of studies utilizing electroencephalography (EEG) to investigate social influence with a peer group has demonstrated that a discrepancy between an individual's opinion and the peer group elicits a fronto-central voltage deviation in the event-related potential (ERP) component known as feedback-related negativity (FRN). The FRN occurs between 200 and 400 milliseconds post-onset of conflicting group feedback, often recorded at the Fz electrode, and is localized to the MFC. It has been proposed that the FRN reflects a neural response akin to a negative prediction error, similar to the activation of the MFC and deactivation of the ventral striatum observed in reinforcement learning models 5,26–28. In magnetoencephalography (MEG) studies of social influence with group opinion electromagnetic brain responses to the disagreement between an individual and group opinion were similar to FRN component with larger negative deflection for conflict trials6,15. A mismatch between individual and group opinions evoked an N400-like component instead of FRN, predominantly involved in conceptual processing and violation of semantic expectations in language studies29,30. In a social influence context, a larger N400 might reflect getting feedback that contradicts an individual's knowledge or beliefs about the world29. Irani and colleagues' study22 with peer and expert feedback showed that conflict with peers compared to conflict with experts in adolescent populations evoked a stronger response in the FRN time window probably related to violation of peer group norms, as the FRN component reflects the automatic detection of errors with sensitivity to the significance of the negative outcome.
Peer and expert opinions affect individuals through distinct underlying motivations. However, when comparing the effects of these influences, it remains unclear which will have a greater impact on the individual. Based on prior evidence, we assumed that expertise could outweigh peer influence when the context involves specialized knowledge. Conversely, peer influence is expected to be more compelling in situations involving social norms, identity, and the need for acceptance. Here, in the domain of fashion preferences, where peer influence is typically strong31, we hypothesize that perceived conflicts between individual and peer group opinions will lead to greater opinion change toward that group’s opinion compared to conflicts with expert opinions. This means that positive and negative discrepancies with peer group opinions are expected to cause an individual’s opinion to shift in a more positive or negative direction, respectively.
Furthermore, we hypothesized that a conflict between an individual's preference and the peer group's opinion would evoke stronger FRN-like neural responses compared to a conflict with the expert opinion as automatic detection of errors and deviation from group norm is often reflected in the FRN component5,6,22,27. In addition to evoked responses, it has been observed that when there is a conflict between an individual’s opinion and the group’s opinion, the associated cognitive and emotional responses are reflected in modulations of neuronal oscillatory activity, specifically theta (4–8 Hz) synchronization and beta (13–30 Hz) desynchronization 6,15. Thus, we predicted that conflict with peer group opinion compared to expert group opinion would increase the power of the theta band and decrease the power of the beta band oscillations.