In our daily communications, we encounter and use expressions inducing the experience of positive and/or negative emotions: we say a compliment, hear encouraging remarks, or read harsh comments. In contrast to negative emotions, positive emotions (e.g., joy, contentment, interest, and love) have been postulated to not only broaden the scope of cognition, attention, and action, but also build an individual’s physical, intellectual, and social resources (e.g., Berridge and Kringelbach, 2015; Fredrickson, 1998). Many previous studies have identified the neural correlates supporting positive emotion processing. These endeavors facilitate crucial insights into the fundamental mechanisms of human affective processing.
Different types of experimental stimuli have been used in previous studies on emotional processing, including scenes, films, pictures, faces, and words associated with various valence (e.g., Schlochtermeier et al., 2013; Seifert, 1997). Compared with emotional pictures and other kinds of stimuli, words allow a high degree of matching in perceptual features, such as word length and frequency (e.g., Laeger et al., 2012). According to the hierarchical emotion theory (Panksepp, 1998, 2005), the valence (i.e., positive or negative) of words is a semantic feature in the lexico-semantic representation (Cato et al., 2004; Jacob et al., 2016; Sylvester et al., 2021). Therefore, emotional words provide a valuable window to explore how language and emotion interact with each other, connecting both linguistic and emotional information (Sylvester et al., 2021). On the one hand, words with affective contents can reliably induce emotion (e.g., Feng et al., 2012; Hamann & Mao, 2002; Schlochtermeier et al., 2013). One the other hand, emotional valence impacts word processing. Indeed, a number of studies on the impact of emotional valence on word processing have shown evidence for processing advantages in emotional words over neutral words, as reflected by higher accuracy rates (e.g., Cacioppo et al., 1999) and more positive event-related potential (ERP) deflections (e.g., Inaba et al., 2005). These findings suggest that emotional processing facilitates lexical processing of emotional words, probably via inducing prioritized access to cognitive resources of perception and attentional control (e.g., Herbert et al., 2009).
With the advancement of the fMRI, investigations on the neural mechanisms of positive emotion processing underscore the crucial roles of some cortical and subcortical regions such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). For example, the mPFC has been found to be activated in response to various emotional stimuli (e.g., pictures, films, words) across different tasks (e.g., emotion valence judgment, recall, lexical decision task) (e.g., Citron, 2012; Phan et al., 2002), and it has been suggested to be responsible for a wide range of emotional processing functions, including detection of emotional signals (Lane et al., 1998), attention to emotion, identification, and evaluation of emotion (e.g., Drevets & Raichle, 1998; Phan et al., 2002). It has also been associated with the self-referential processing of emotional stimuli, such as online monitoring and subjective evaluation of one’s emotional feelings and experience (Fossati et al., 2003; Herbert et al., 2011; Jäncke, & Brühl, 2010; Jenkins & Mitchell, 2010; Lee and Siegle, 2009). Specific to positive emotion processing, the medial PFC (mPFC) has consistently exhibited greater activations for positive stimuli, as compared to neutral stimuli (e.g., Anders et al., 2004; Kensinger & Schacter, 2006; Lane et al., 1997). The activation of this region is in line with the hierarchical emotion theory (Panksepp, 1998, 2005), which posits that valence is located at the tertiary process level supported by neocortical areas, such as the middle frontal cortex.
In addition, the PCC has been found to respond to the emotional arousal ratings of the stimuli, and it has been linked to evaluating the emotional significance of sensory stimuli (e.g., Cato et al., 2004; Heilman et al., 1993; Maddock, 1999, 2003; Maddock & Buonocore, 1997; Posner et al., 2009). For example, Maddock et al. (2003) reported stronger activation in the PCC when participants evaluated valance of positive words than neutral words. Indeed, the PCC has been identified as an important region for implicit affective network (e.g., Briesemeister et al. 2015; Sylvester et al., 2021), modulating processes which do not require explicit emotion evaluation, induction, or regulation, such as lexical decision. This region has also been associated with retrieval of episodic memory of past emotional experience (e.g., Cananna & Trimble, 2006; Foland-Ross et al., 2013; Kuchinke et al., 2005; Mantani et al., 2005; Maratos et al., 2001).
Furthermore, the co-activation of the PFC and PCC has been highlighted in some studies (e.g., Braem et al., 2021; Maddock et al., 2003), which suggest that these two regions may form a network and collaboratively contribute to emotional processing. One possibility is that the PCC receives efferent emotional information from the PFC and transmits it to its adjacent limbic structures (e.g., Posner et al., 2009). Another possibility is that the PCC is activated earlier than conscious cognitive processing (e.g., Bentley et al., 2003; Frot et al., 2008; Vogt, 2014), responsible for preconscious evaluation of the valence of the incoming stimuli (Anderson & Phelps, 2001; Windmann et al., 2002), as posited by the affective primacy hypothesis (Murphy & Zajone, 1993). As the PFC has been proposed to subserve top-down conscious control of emotion (e.g., Laeger et al., 2012) and to be a key component recruited at the tertiary process level for valence processing (Panksepp, 1998, 2008), it is possible that it is mainly responsible for the conscious semantic processing of the emotional contents of words. More investigations are needed to test this hypothesis. It has also been suggested that the PCC and mPFC are key components of the DMN network responsible for self-reflection and self-relevance appraisal (e.g., Foland-Ross et al., 2013) and the cortical midline structures critical for active emotional modulation (e.g., Herbert et al., 2011; Ochsner et al., 2004).
Interestingly, specific to positive emotional word processing, the mPFC and the PCC have not been consistently found to be responsive to word valence. Firstly, for the mPFC, some studies (e.g., Herbert et al., 2011; Maddock et al., 2003) reported greater neural responses to positive emotional words than matched control words, while others (e.g., Cato et al., 2004; Kensinger & Schacter, 2006) did not observe such a pattern. Similarly, the previous findings regarding the PCC are also mixed: it was reported to be activated to a greater extent during positive word processing, relative to neutral word processing in some studies (e.g., Maddock et al., 2003; Sylvester et al. 2021), but not in others (e.g., Hamann & Mao, 2002; Inaba et al., 2005). Furthermore, some other regions have been independently reported to be responsive to valence of positive words, including the temporal cortex (Briesemeister et al., 2015), the anterior cingulate cortex (Schlochtermeier et al., 2013), the inferior frontal cortex (Kensinger & Schacter, 2006), and the rostral frontal and retrosplenial Cortices (Cato et al., 2004).
The above-mentioned discrepancies could probably be attributed to varying sample sizes, experimental design, data processing, and statistical analyses. First of all, small sample sizes are likely to cause high volatility in results and lack of statistical significance. To obtain more reliable findings, larger sample sizes are needed to test whether previously reported results could be replicated. Additionally, most prior studies used a single experimental paradigm in a certain context, and the divergences in findings could be linked to task-dependent and contextual factors. Therefore, it is important to further test the reliability of results based on a range of tasks across various contexts. Lastly, differences in data processing and statistical analyses across studies may also contribute to the divergences in brain regions reported.
Combining related findings from multiple studies, meta-analysis provides an effective means to reveal more consistent and reliable results across studies (Gurevitch et al., 2018). In a recent meta-analysis study, Dang et al. (2023) have identified six key regions that are involved in the processing of negative words: the left medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), the left posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), the left amygdala, the left inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), and the left thalamus. Also, Feng et al. (2023) conducted a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies using implicit emotional tasks (e.g., an emotional Stroop task) involving negative words (and pictures) and found that negative words engaged the default mode network (DMN, the PCC, mPFC, and inferior parietal lobule) and the frontal-parietal network (the ventrolateral PFC, DLPFC, and the dorsal mPFC). They suggested that these networks were associated with top-down negative emotion regulation and semantic processing. To the best of our knowledge, there is a lack of meta-analyses on positive word processing. It is important to pinpoint the brain regions consistently shown to support positive emotional words to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the neural networks of human affective processing. Such converging and consistent evidence based on quantitative examinations would serve as a significant prerequisite for making appropriate generalization about brain functions (Fusar-Poli et al., 2019). Theoretically, such investigations would also shed light on the laterality of positive emotion processing. According to the valence hypothesis (Davidson, 1995), the positive stimuli would be predominantly processed in the left hemisphere, which received support from some studies (e.g., Canli et al., 1998). In contrast, the right hemisphere hypothesis assumes that the right hemisphere is dominant for processing emotions, whereas the left hemisphere is dominant for processing language (e.g., Gainotti, 1997; Cato et al., 2004). A meta-analysis approach of positive emotional words would provide reliable and strong evidence to answer the laterality question of processing positive words, where language and emotion connect with each other.
In Study 1, we aimed to conduct two separate meta-analyses for previous fMRI studies on positive emotional words by using activation likelihood estimation (ALE) (Chein et al., 2002; Turkeltaub et al., 2002) and Seed-based d Mapping with Permutation of Subject Images (SDM-PSI) (Albajes-Eizagirre et al., 2019), respectively. Based on data from multiple studies, Meta-analyses can effectively increase the sample sizes and improve the reliability and replicability of results, overcoming the heterogeneity caused by the small sample sizes. Studies using an array of emotional word processing tasks (e.g., emotion naming or valence evaluation) were included in order to minimize the modulations of task-dependent and contextual factors and underscore the commonality of previous results. Such investigations would generate additional insights into the neural mechanisms of emotional processing.
As an objective quantitative data analysis technique, the ALE meta-analysis has been widely used in coordinate-based neuroimaging data (Dang et al., 2023; Hartwigsen et al., 2019; McTeague et al., 2020). Modeling the coordinates of activation foci in the studies, ALE evaluates the overlap among foci based on probability distribution centered at the coordinates. Apart from ALE meta-analysis, we also conducted a seed-based d mapping (SDM, Albajes-Eizagirre et al, 2019; Albajes-Eizagirre & Radua, 2018) meta-analysis, which is an alternative coordinate-based random-effects approach. Combining the peak coordinates with their statistical parameters, SDM assesses the weight (significance) of the peak coordinates in each study based on more indicators than the ALE meta-analysis. Recently, SDM has been applied to an increasing number of fMRI meta-analyses in different research fields, such as language studies (Li & Bi, 2022; Cheng et al., 2023; Zhang et al., 2023), emotional processing (Liu et al., 2022; Zhou et al., 2020), social cognition (Maliske et al., 2023; Schurz et al., 2021), and cognitive and neurological disorders (Clements et al., 2018; Hart et al., 2013; Luijten et al., 2017; Radua et al., 2015). Different from ALE, SDM conducts quantitative statistical analyses based on the effect size map of each experiment and reveals the modulations of covariates. Some prior meta-analysis studies have reached relatively stable and consistent results based on a smaller number (n ≤ 10) of experiments (Liu et al., 2022; Li & Bi, 2022; Yan et al, 2021), as compared with the requirement of a larger number (n ≥ 17) of experiments for ALE analysis.
ALE and SDM meta-analyses are different in a variety of ways and thus complement each other. First of all, the ALE analysis does not distinguish positive and negative differences, whereas the SDM analysis accounts for voxel activations and deactivations (Radua et al., 2012). Thus, the SDM analysis effectively addressed the opposite effects reported in different studies. Secondly, the ALE analysis considers the total number of peaks and thus cannot be weighted by sample size, while the SDM analysis addresses this issue by separating peaks of individual studies (Radua & Mataix-Cols, 2012). Additionally, ALE estimates the peak likelihood, while SDM estimates the effect size (Radua et al., 2014). It has been indicated that ALE offers a broader view for the researcher to examine and interpret results (Zhang et al., 2023). Conducting both the ALE and SDM meta-analyses, we aimed to test whether the regions involved in processing positive words calculated by the two approaches would be consistent for ensuring high reliability or results.
Moreover, we are interested in the universality of the brain regions revealed by meta-analyses across different languages. Thus, in Study 2, we first examined the neural mechanisms for processing positive words in two different native languages (Chinese and English). Then, we investigated the neural mechanisms of Chinese-English bilinguals when they processed positive words in their first language (L1) and second language (L2). Such investigations would further test the reliability of meta-analyses results in Study 1 and provide constructive insights for the fundamental theoretical inquiries of where neuroanatomically positive words are processed across different languages in native speakers and in L1 and L2 within the bilingual brain. The explorations in bilinguals would unveil the potential overlap and differences in neural substrates supporting positive word processing across their two languages.
Study 1 ALE and SDM Meta-analyses of positive words to identify ROIs