2.1 L2 WTC
McCroskey and Baer (1985) first introduced the concept of WTC for communication in the first language (L1), and subsequently MacIntyre and colleagues introduced it to the field of L2 acquisition (MacIntyre and Charos, 1996), and defined it as a learner’s “readiness to enter into discourse at a particular time with a specific person or persons, using a L2” (MacIntyre et al., 1998: 547). Believed to be “an antecedent of the L2 learners’ actual communicative behaviors” (Lan et al., 2021: 3), it allows L2 learners to “cultivate their communicative competence, and proceed with their L2 acquisition process effectively (Li, 2024: 2). To promote studies on L2 WTC, MacIntyre et al. (1998) developed a heuristic model of L2 WTC, in which various personal, interpersonal, and situational factors that may influence learners’ L2 WTC were encapsulated.
This theoretical model comprises six layers. The first layer is communicative behavior, referring to the specific behaviors of an individual engaging in L2 communication in real-life situations. The frequency, quality, and diversity of communicative behaviors can all directly reflect an individual’s level of L2 WTC. The second layer is behavioral intention, which precedes communicative behavior and serves as the internal impetus driving an individual to initiate or engage in L2 communicative acts. The stronger the behavioral intention, the more likely an individual is to initiate or engage in L2 communication in real-life situations. The third layer consists of situational antecedents that have a direct impact upon L2 WTC, including one’s desire to communicate with a specific person, and his/her state of communicative self-confidence. In all, the factors in these layers of the model, being subject to change per se depending on the specific situation or context, have a direct and short-term impact upon a person’s L2 WTC at any given moment. The fourth to sixth layers encompass enduring factors that indirectly affect L2 WTC, including one’s interpersonal and intergroup motivation, intergroup attitudes and climate, social situation, personality, among others, which represent “distal, enduring, and broadly applicable influences on L2 communication” (MacIntyre and Wang, 2021: 4). Taken together, it could be argued that the model proposed by MacIntyre et al. (1998) integrates various factors relating to language, communication, society, psychology, and personality, offering a multidimensional and holistic perspective on the comprehensive influences on WTC in L2 communication contexts (Shi and Fan, 2022).
Inspired by this model, a spate of inquiries have sought to validate potential predictors of L2 WTC, including learning motivation (especially within the L2 motivational self system framework), language mindset, emotional intelligence, class social atmosphere/classroom environment, along with studies on the influence of language attitude, international posture, and global perspective (see Li, 2024: 2). However, the personality factors (e.g. grit) within L2 WTC have received relatively less attention in existing research, especially in a Chinese EFL context. Moreover, in most prior studies, L2 WTC has been treated as a collective variable, but some researchers have suggested an obvious distinction between meaning-focused and form-focused L2 WTC (e.g., Li and Li, 2022; Peng and Woodrow, 2010; Weaver, 2005). While meaning-focused L2 WTC signifies willingness to communicate in an L2 with the teacher or the entire class for the exchange of ideas, thoughts and feelings, form-focused L2 WTC involves willingness to communicate in the L2 with a smaller, more familiar audience, such as nearby peers or classmates for solutions to language-related problems (Li and Li, 2022: 19). Given this difference, it is interesting to know whether grit exerts differential effects on the two types of L2 WTC. Therefore, this study tries to explore the influence of grit, a personality factor located in the bottom layer of MacIntyre et al.’s (1998) model, on Chinese undergraduate English majors’ L2 WTC in both meaning-focused and form-focused activities of communication.
2.2 Grit
The concept of grit, introduced by Duckworth and colleagues seventeen years ago, has garnered considerable interest within the psychological community. Defined as “perseverance and passion for long-term goals” (Duckworth et al., 2007: 1087), it is believed to have a stronger predictive effect on achievement compared to IQ and aptitude (Liu et al., 2022). Indeed, previous social psychological research has found that grit is an important predictor of success across diverse academic and non-academic domains among different populations (Teimouri et al., 2022). Generally, grit is regarded as a higher-order factor encompassing two dimensions: perseverance of effort (PE) and consistency of interest (CI) (Duckworth et al., 2007; Duckworth and Quinn, 2009). PE denotes the extent to which an individual persists and maintains efforts in order to achieve their long-term objectives, while CI refers to the degree to which an individual maintains interest and enthusiasm in achieving their long-term goals, regardless of the challenges and failures they may encounter (Sudina et al., 2021).
Despite the pivotal role of grit in achieving success, research exploring the role of grit in the process of language learning is relatively limited (Keegan, 2017; Mikami, 2024; Sudina et al., 2021), with studies on the relationship between grit and L2 WTC emerging only in recent years (e.g., Ebn-Abbasi et al., 2022; Ebn-Abbasi et al., 2024; Lee, 2020; Lee & Chen Hsieh, 2019; Lee & Drajati, 2019; Wang, 2023; Yang et al., 2024). For example, Lee & Chen Hsieh (2019) examined the relationship between emotional variables (grit, L2 confidence, L2 motivation, L2 anxiety) and L2 WTC in in-class, out-of-class, and digital contexts among 261 undergraduate students from different majors in Taiwan. The results suggested that students with higher grit and L2 confidence displayed higher L2 WTC in all three communicative settings. In a recent study, Yang et al. (2024) probed into the connections between teacher support, grit, and L2 WTC among a sample of 619 Chinese university students, and the results indicated that grit could directly and positively predict L2 WTC, and indirectly affect it through the mediation of foreign language enjoyment.
However, in most of the previous studies on the relationship between grit and L2 WTC, the construct of grit is taken as a collective variable, although differences of its two dimensions of PE and CI has been noted in the literature (e.g., Alamer, 2021; Credé et al., 2017; Muenks et al., 2017; Sudina et al., 2021). For example, Credé et al.’s (2017) meta-analysis, involving 88 independent samples of 66,807 individuals, revealed that PE is a more powerful predictor of performance than CI. Sudina et al. (2021) found that PE positively predicted L2 proficiency in an EFL context while CI had a negative predictive effect in an English-as-a-second-language (ESL) context. Based on these findings, in the present study, the two dimensions of grit, viz. PE and CI, were taken as separate variables in order to reveal possible differences in their predictive effect.
2.3 Foreign Language Enjoyment and Anxiety
Emotional factors in L2 acquisition have long been regarded as “irrational factors” and therefore largely neglected until the introduction of humanistic perspectives on language teaching in the 1970s and 1980s, which prompted scholars to consider language learners holistically by assigning equal importance to cognitive and emotional factors (Li and Xu, 2019).
Further, the rise and swift advancement of positive psychology in general education (e.g., Lake, 2013; MacIntyre and Gregersen, 2012; Mercer and MacIntyre, 2014) have prompted scholars to broaden their attention from solely negative emotions such as boredom, anxiety, and burnout to both positive and negative emotions in L2 teaching and learning (Wang et al., 2021). A range of studies have found that language learners and users experience a range of emotions in various settings, with foreign language enjoyment (FLE) and anxiety (FLA) being the most common emotions in the classroom (Li and Xu, 2019).
The notion of FLE was first introduced to L2 acquisition by Dewaele and MacIntyre (2014: 242) and was described as “enjoyment, fun, interest, and lack of boredom”. According to Li et al. (2018), FLE is a positive feeling derived from surpassing homeostatic constraints and pushing oneself to achieve something novel or unanticipated, particularly when confronted with challenging tasks. The notion of FLA was first introduced to L2 acquisition by Horwitz et al. (1986). They posited that L2 anxiety differs from general anxiety in that it possesses a certain degree of language specificity, referring to the complex self-perceptions, beliefs, feelings, and behaviors associated with foreign language learning. It is represented by the three sub-levels of communication apprehension, test anxiety, and fear of negative evaluation.
FLE and FLA, as two related but distinct and fundamentally independent emotions in L2 learning (Wang and Ren, 2024), have been shown to have a clear pattern of relationships with L2 achievement, with a significant positive relationship between FLE and L2 achievement, and significant negative relationship between FLA and L2 achievement (Li and Xu, 2019; Wang and Ren, 2024), although there are some different findings related to FLA (see Yu 2022, 2024). In recent years, the role of FLE and FLA in shaping learners’ L2 WTC has also been under scrutiny. For example, Wang et al. (2024), in a study involving 547 undergraduate students learning languages other than English in the United States, found that students’ L2 WTC in class was associated with their anxiety, boredom and enjoyment, and that these emotions along with pride mediated the relationships between classroom social climate/language mindset and L2 WTC. Wei and Chen (2022), in their study of a sample of 622 Chinese undergraduate non-English majors, found that FLE and FLA significantly positively and negatively predicted L2 WTC, respectively, and that both of them mediated the relationships between growth/fixed language mindset and L2 WTC. However, as noted earlier, in these studies, the construct of grit was taken as a single collective variable, and no distinction was made between meaning-focused and form-focused L2 WTC. Therefore, the roles of FLE and FLA in the relationships between PE/CI and meaning-focused/ form-focused L2 WTC remain unexplored. Moreover, FLE can be derived from different sources, such as individuals themselves, the atmosphere or environment of L2 learning, and the L2 teacher (Li et al., 2018). An interesting question that arises is whether these sources of FLE exhibit different relationships with the two dimensions of grit and the two types of L2 WTC. With these considerations, the current study seeks to probe into the direct influence of PE and CI on meaning-focused and form-focused L2 WTC, and the possible mediating effects of FLA and the three sources of FLE in their relationship.