Sex differences occur in brain substructure atrophy during aging with females showing relative protection at midlife followed by abrupt volume loss thereafter
First, we assessed how aging affects spatial reference memory using Morris Water Maze (MWM) testing at young (3-4 months), midlife (12-14 months), and old (20-22 months) ages in females and male C57Bl/6 mice. There was no significant impairment of spatial memory in either females or males with aging (Fig. 1a, b, c, d). Then, we determined if a biomarker of brain aging, namely substructure atrophy, might be more sensitive in detecting neurodegeneration. We collected in vivo MR images in both female and male mice at the same three ages: young, midlife, and old and used atlas-based morphometry to analyze substructure volumes (Fig. 1e). Both females and males showed significant atrophy at old age as compared to young in frontal cortex and striatum (Fig. 1f, g). However, at midlife there was a sex difference. Females had relative protection as compared to males. The trajectory in males was gradual atrophy from young to midlife to old ages. In contrast, females showed no significant atrophy from young to midlife, but thereafter had an abrupt drop in substructure volumes from midlife to old age. Furthermore, the dorsal hippocampus is primarily involved in cognition and memory and is analogous to the posterior hippocampus in humans which is known to atrophy with age39,40. In whole hippocampus, as well as dorsal hippocampus, female mice had no atrophy from young to midlife, but had significant atrophy at old age (Fig. 1h). Two-way ANOVA indicated a significant interaction between sex and age in dorsal hippocampus (p = 0.0059). This was region-specific in that neither females nor males showed atrophy in ventral hippocampus with age (not shown). Thus, females were relatively protected against aging associated region-specific atrophy at midlife, but thereafter the trajectory of atrophy was striking.
Loss of endogenous ovarian hormones induces hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment and dorsal hippocampus atrophy at midlife in females
We next determined the effect of loss of endogenous female and male hormones on cognitive behavioral testing and regional brain atrophy by in vivo MRI in females and males at midlife. Gonadectomy (GDX) versus sham surgery occurred at two months of age with cognitive testing in young and midlife mice. This revealed another sex difference. GDX females showed impairment on the MWM test of spatial reference memory at midlife, while GDX males performed well at midlife (Fig. 2a, b). An interaction between loss of ovarian hormones and aging in females was then discovered. GDX females at midlife had impairment, but GDX females that were young performed well (Fig. 2c, d). Thus, spatial memory impairment was due to both loss of ovarian hormones and aging.
To further investigate hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment induced by loss of endogenous ovarian hormones in females at midlife, we performed Y-maze testing to assess working memory. There was a significant decrease in percent spontaneous alteration in GDX females at midlife compared to gonadally intact (sham) at midlife, and there was also a decrease in GDX females at midlife compared to GDX females that were young (Fig 2e). This result extended the observation of cognitive impairment in GDX females at midlife from spatial reference memory to working memory. Lastly, we addressed contextual fear conditioning. In concordance with the MWM and Y-maze tasks, GDX females at midlife showed a significant decrease in percent total freezing time 24 hours after conditioning compared to gonadally intact females at midlife (Fig 2f). These three hippocampal-dependent behavioral tasks demonstrated that loss of endogenous ovarian hormones in females induced cognitive impairment at midlife.
We next determined the effect of removal of endogenous ovarian hormones in females on region-specific brain atrophy at midlife, focusing on the hippocampus and its substructures. In vivo MR images from GDX and sham female mice were collected at midlife and old age. In gonadally intact female mice at old age, there was atrophy in dorsal hippocampus, with a trend in whole hippocampus, and no atrophy in ventral hippocampus (Fig 2g, h, i). At midlife, GDX females compared to gonadally intact demonstrated atrophy of dorsal hippocampus, with a trend in whole hippocampus, and no atrophy in ventral hippocampus (Fig. 2g, h, i). Together this revealed region-specific neuroprotective effects, whereby endogenous ovarian hormones in females conferred neuroprotection against atrophy of the dorsal hippocampus at midlife.
Dorsal hippocampal atrophy as measured by in vivo MRI in aging mice is a biomarker for hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment
To determine the relationship between hippocampal atrophy and spatial reference memory, we correlated hippocampal volumes with time spent in the target quadrant (TQ) on the MWM. We observed that there was a direct correlation between whole hippocampal volume and time spent in TQ (r = 0.22, p = 0.049). Smaller volumes (worse atrophy) in the hippocampus were associated with worse spatial reference memory performance. Correlations using hippocampal substructures demonstrated that this effect in whole hippocampus was driven by a direct correlation between dorsal hippocampus volume and time spent in TQ (Fig. 2j), with no correlation between ventral hippocampus volume and time spent in TQ (Fig. 2). This demonstrated that dorsal hippocampal atrophy as measured by in vivo MRI in aging mice is a sensitive biomarker for hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment. This correlation with function underscored the importance of a region-specific approach in evaluating brain atrophy during aging.
Dorsal hippocampal neuropathology
Given that atrophy of dorsal hippocampus was induced by GDX at midlife in females, we next assessed the effect of GDX on neuropathology within dorsal hippocampus. Reactive astrogliosis was determined using GFAP expression. There was an increase in GFAP+ astrocytes in GDX females at midlife compared to gonadally intact (sham) at midlife. There was also an increase in GDX females at midlife compared to GDX females that were young (Fig. 3a, b). Next, microglia activation was assessed by colocalization of Iba1 and MHCII. There was a trend for an increase in microglia activation in GDX females at midlife compared to gonadally intact, and there was a significant increase in GDX females at midlife compared to GDX females that were young (Fig. 3c). Lastly, synaptic loss was assessed using the post-synaptic marker PSD95. GDX females at midlife showed significant synaptic loss as compared to GDX females that were young. Gonadally intact females at midlife as compared to gonadally intact females that were young also showed some synaptic loss, but to a milder degree (Fig. 3d).
We next examined the relationship between dorsal hippocampus neuropathologies. Astrogliosis and microglia activation showed a direct correlation (r = 0.577, p = 0.0020) with each other. There was an indirect correlation of PSD95+ area fraction with GFAP+ area fraction (r = -0.4374, p = 0.0199) and with microglia activation (r = -0.7772, p < 0.0001). Thus, synaptic loss correlated with increased astrogliosis and microglia activation. To address functional relevance, a cross-modality correlation analysis determined the relationship between dorsal hippocampus neuropathology and performance on the MWM test. Interestingly, there was a direct correlation between PSD95+ area fraction and time spent in the TQ (Fig. 3e), and conversely an indirect correlation between PSD95+ area fraction and time spent in the other three quadrants (Fig 3f). Finally, regarding the relationship between glial pathology and function, there was an inverse correlation between time spent in the TQ and astrogliosis (r = -0.425, p = 0.024) and microglia activation (r = -0.4838, p = 0.0166). Thus, glial activation and synaptic loss in dorsal hippocampus correlated with worse spatial reference memory function.
Together, this demonstrates that loss of endogenous ovarian hormones in females induces glia activation and synaptic loss in the dorsal hippocampus, atrophy of dorsal hippocampus by in vivo MRI, and hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment at midlife, with significant relationships between each.
Neuroprotection in females at midlife is mediated by ERb in astrocytes.
Next, we investigated a region-specific, cell-specific, and receptor-specific mechanism through which endogenous estrogens could be mediating neuroprotection against hippocampal-dependent cognitive impairment and dorsal hippocampal atrophy at midlife. Hippocampal astrocytes play a role in memory formation and regulation of synaptic transmission, and ERβ is expressed in both neurons and astrocytes in hippocampus29,30,41,42. Focus on ERβ was supported by translational potential since a treatment targeting ERβ activation would not confer ERα mediated adverse effects on breast. Thus, we determined whether ERβ signaling in either neurons or astrocytes could mediate the effects of endogenous estrogens on hippocampal-dependent cognition and dorsal hippocampal atrophy. To test this, we created estrogen receptor b (ERb) conditional knock-outs in astrocytes (astrocyte ERb CKO) or neurons (neuron ERb CKO). At midlife, gonadally intact mice with selective deletion of either ERb in astrocytes or neurons, as well as wild type (WT) littermates, underwent cognitive behavioral testing and in vivo MRI. In the MWM task, mice with deletion of ERβ in astrocytes showed a significant reduction of percent time in the TQ, compared to WT littermates (Fig. 4a). In contrast, deletion of ERβ in neurons showed no impairment of spatial reference memory (Fig. 4a). Indeed, both WT and neuron ERb CKO females at midlife showed highly significant preference for the TQ, compared to other quadrants (Fig. 4b), while astrocyte ERβ CKO females did not show this preference for the TQ (Fig 4b). Regarding contextual fear conditioning, astrocyte ERβ CKO females at midlife showed a significant decrease in percent total freezing time at 24-hours, compared to WT littermates (Fig. 4c). In contrast, mice with deletion of ERβ in neurons showed no impairment of contextual fear conditioning as compared to WT littermates (Fig. 4c). Notably, females at midlife with selective deletion of ERα in astrocytes had no impairment of contextual fear conditioning (Supplemental Fig. 1).
We next determined whether a protective effect of endogenous estrogens on hippocampal atrophy in females at midlife could be mediated through ERb in astrocytes or neurons. Interestingly, astrocyte ERb CKO mice showed atrophy in the hippocampus compared to WT littermates, while neuron ERb CKO mice did not (Fig. 4d). When we examined hippocampal substructure volumes, we found that atrophy in the dorsal hippocampus was primarily driving the effect in whole hippocampus. The dorsal hippocampus was significantly smaller in astrocyte ERb CKO compared to WT mice, while there was no difference between neuron ERb CKO and WT mice (Fig. 4e). In ventral hippocampus, astrocyte ERb CKO had a slightly smaller volume compared to WT, but there was no difference between astrocyte ERb CKO and neuron ERb CKO (Fig. 4f).
Since the Cre-system we utilized entailed selective deletion of ERβ in astrocytes or neurons throughout development and adulthood, we next addressed whether cognitive impairment or hippocampal atrophy had been induced during development. This was not the case. Young (age 3-4 month) female astrocyte ERβ CKO, as compared to WT littermates, had no deficits in spatial reference memory (Supplemental Fig. 2a, b) or 24-hour contextual fear memory (Supplemental Fig. 2c). Further, there was no hippocampal atrophy in young astrocyte ERβ CKO as compared to young WT littermates (Supplemental Fig. 2d, e, f).
Selective deletion of ERβ in astrocytes, but not neurons, induced impairment in remote memory.
Recent memory formation is hippocampal-dependent. Persistence of memory undergoes a transition from hippocampal-dependence for recent memories to hippocampal-independence for remote memories, through system memory consolidation. Medial prefrontal cortex supports remote memories, initially formed in hippocampus, by retrieving memory at remote time-points (28 days later)43,44. Thus, we next determined if loss of endogenous ovarian hormones affects remote memory. Remote contextual fear memory was evaluated in GDX versus gonadally intact (sham) females at midlife using the 1-month contextual fear memory test. GDX females at midlife showed a trend for reduction of percent total freezing time at 1-month compared to sham females (Supplemental Fig. 3a). Further, gonadally intact astrocyte ERβ CKO females showed a significant decrease in percent total freezing time at 1-month compared to WT littermate females (Supplemental Fig. 3b), while neuron ERβ CKO females did not. Together, these findings demonstrate an essential role for ERβ in astrocytes in females at midlife on recent and remote memory.